Comparing the Efficacy of Two Forms of Self-Affirmation to Reduce Stereotype Threat Effects on Women’s Math Performance

被引:0
作者
Macchione A.L. [1 ]
Sacco D. [1 ]
机构
[1] The University of Southern Mississippi, Owings McQuagge Hall (OMH), Golden Eagle Avenue, Hattiesburg, 39401, MS
关键词
Leaky pipeline; Math performance; Problem-solving; Self-affirmation; Stereotype threat;
D O I
10.1007/s41979-023-00085-x
中图分类号
学科分类号
摘要
Research demonstrates that when math-based gender stereotypes are activated (e.g., men are better at math than women), women display comparatively poorer math performance than men, a phenomenon referred to as stereotype threat. We evaluated the effectiveness of two forms of self-affirmation in reducing the effects of stereotype threat on women’s math performance. Participants completed a math test under one of four conditions: control (no explicit stereotype activation), stereotype threat (activation of gender performance stereotype), or stereotype threat combined with one of two self-affirmation manipulations. Women in the affirmation conditions either read about women’s greater verbal or relational ability and were asked to write about why the trait is important to their self-concept. No omnibus effect of condition emerged though follow-up analyses revealed several notable findings. While we were unable to replicate stereotype threat effects, contrast analyses revealed that the combined performance of women in the two affirmation conditions was greater than the combined performance of women in the two no-affirmation conditions. Women in the relational affirmation condition performed descriptively greater than the combined performance of women in the other three conditions. These findings demonstrate how self-affirmation, particularly relational affirmation, may facilitate women’s mathematics problem-solving, independent of stereotype threat activation. © 2023, The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Nature Switzerland AG.
引用
收藏
页码:181 / 198
页数:17
相关论文
共 59 条
[31]  
Kimura D., Sex differences in the brain, Scientific American, 267, 3, pp. 118-125, (1992)
[32]  
Koole S.L., Smeets K., Van Knippenberg A., Dijksterhuis A., The cessation of rumination through self-affirmation, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology., 77, pp. 111-125, (1999)
[33]  
Women Outperform Men in 11 of 12 Key Emotional Intelligence Competencies., (2016)
[34]  
Levy B., Improving memory in old age through implicit self-stereotyping, Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 71, 6, pp. 1092-1107, (1996)
[35]  
Lindberg S.M., Hyde J.S., Petersen J.L., Linn M.C., New trends in gender and mathematics performance: A meta-analysis, Psychological Bulletin, 136, 6, pp. 1123-1135, (2010)
[36]  
Martens A., Johns M., Greenberg J., Schimel J., Combating stereotype threat: The effect of self-affirmation on women’s intellectual performance, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, pp. 236-243, (2006)
[37]  
Master A., Meltzoff A., Building bridges between psychological science and education: Cultural stereotypes, STEM, and equity, Prospects, 46, pp. 1-20, (2017)
[38]  
Mikulincer M., Mental rumination and learned helplessness: Cognitive shifts during helplessness training and their behavioral consequences, Cognitive Interference: Theories, Methods, and Findings, pp. 191-209, (1996)
[39]  
Science faculty’s subtle gender biases favor male students, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, (2012)
[40]  
Nosek B.A., Smyth F.L., Sriram N., Lindner N.M., Devos T., Ayala A., Et al., National differences in gender–science stereotypes predict national sex differences in science and math achievement, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 26, pp. 10593-10597, (2009)