Gestures of Despair and Hope: A View on Deliberate Self-harm From Economics and Evolutionary Biology

被引:30
作者
Hagen E.H. [1 ]
Watson P.J. [2 ]
Hammerstein P. [3 ]
机构
[1] Department of Anthropology, Washington State University, Vancouver, BC
[2] Department of Biology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM
[3] Institute for Theoretical Biology, Humboldt University, Berlin
关键词
bargaining; costly signaling; Darwinian medicine; Darwinian psychiatry; deliberate self-harm; honest signaling; parasuicide;
D O I
10.1162/biot.2008.3.2.123
中图分类号
学科分类号
摘要
A long-standing theoretical tradition in clinical psychology and psychiatry sees deliberate self-harm (DSH), such as wrist-cutting, as “functional”—a means to avoid painful emotions, for example, or to elicit attention from others. There is substantial evidence that DSH serves these functions. Yet the specific links between self-harm and such functions remain obscure. Why don’t self-harmers use less destructive behaviors to blunt painful emotions or elicit attention? Economists and biologists have used game theory to show that, under certain circumstances, self-harmful behaviors by economic agents and animals serve important strategic goals. In particular, “costly signals” can credibly reveal a “private state” in situations where verbal claims and other “cheap” signals might be disbelieved. Here, DSH is scrutinized using signaling theory, and a variant, the theory of bargaining with private information. The social contexts and associated features of DSH suggest that it might be a costly, and therefore credible, signal of need that compels social partners to respond. © 2008, Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognition Research.
引用
收藏
页码:123 / 138
页数:15
相关论文
共 89 条
  • [71] Skegg K., Nada-Raja S., Moffit T.E., Minor self-harm and psychiatric disorder: A population-based study, Suicide and Life-threatening Behavior, 34, pp. 187-196, (2004)
  • [72] Slavin M.O., The origins of psychic conflict and the adaptive function of repression: An evolutionary biological view, Psychoanalysis and Contemporary Thought, 8, pp. 407-440, (1985)
  • [73] Sourander A., Aromaa M., Pihlakoski L., Haavisto A., Rautava P., Helenius H., Sillanpaa M., Early predictors of deliberate self-harm among adolescents: A prospective follow-up study from age 3 to age 15, Journal of Affective Disorders, 93, pp. 87-96, (2006)
  • [74] Spence M., Job market signaling, Quarterly Journal of Economics, 81, pp. 355-374, (1973)
  • [75] Starrett A., Adaptive resemblance: A unifying concept for mimicry and crypsis, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 48, pp. 299-317, (1993)
  • [76] Suyemoto K.L., The functions of self-mutilation, Clinical Psychology Review, 18, pp. 531-554, (1998)
  • [77] Szamado S., Szathmary E., Selective scenarios for the emergence of natural language, Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 21, pp. 555-561, (2006)
  • [78] Trivers R.L., Parent-offspring conflict, American Zoologist, 14, pp. 249-264, (1974)
  • [79] Trivers R.L., Social Evolution, (1985)
  • [80] Tulloch A.L., Blizzard L., Pinkus Z., Adolescent-parent communication in self-harm, Journal of Adolescent Health, 21, pp. 267-275, (1997)