Influence of the constituent morpheme boundary on compound word access

被引:0
|
作者
Alexander Taikh
Christina L. Gagné
Thomas L. Spalding
机构
[1] Concordia University of Edmonton,Department of Psychology
[2] University of Alberta,Department of Psychology
来源
Memory & Cognition | 2024年 / 52卷
关键词
Morphological decomposition; Masked priming; Compound words; Transposed letter priming effect;
D O I
暂无
中图分类号
学科分类号
摘要
Embedded morphemes are thought to become available during the processing of multi-morphemic words, and impact access to the whole word. According to the edge-aligned embedded word activation theory Grainger & Beyersmann, (2017), embedded morphemes receive activation when the whole word can be decomposed into constituent morphemes. Thus, interfering with morphological decomposition also interferes with access to the embedded morphemes. Numerous studies have examined the effects of interfering with boundary and constituent-internal letters on morphological decomposition by comparing the effect of transposing letters at the morphemic boundary to constituent-internal letters. These studies, which report inconsistent findings, have typically used derived multi-morphemic words (e.g., cleaner), and sometimes use a control replacement letter condition that is not matched to the transposed letter conditions in terms of location. Across five experiments, we test the edge-aligned activation theory by examining the effects of replacing and transposing boundary and constituent-internal letters of compounds. Our findings suggest that replacing boundary letters interferes with access to both embedded constituents, while replacing constituent-internal letters still allows for access to the unaltered constituent, thus compensating for the interference in the altered constituent. Our findings are consistent with the edge-aligned theory with respect to letter replacement, and also imply that letter replacement must match the position of letter transposition when it is used as a control condition.
引用
收藏
页码:680 / 723
页数:43
相关论文
共 37 条
  • [21] CompLex: an eye-movement database of compound word reading in English
    Daniel Schmidtke
    Julie A. Van Dyke
    Victor Kuperman
    Behavior Research Methods, 2021, 53 : 59 - 77
  • [22] CompLex: an eye-movement database of compound word reading in English
    Schmidtke, Daniel
    Van Dyke, Julie A.
    Kuperman, Victor
    BEHAVIOR RESEARCH METHODS, 2021, 53 (01) : 59 - 77
  • [23] Time course of Chinese compound word recognition as revealed by ERP data
    Wang, Yuling
    Li, Zuowen
    Jiang, Minghu
    Long, Fei
    Huang, Yunlong
    Xu, Xinyi
    LANGUAGE COGNITION AND NEUROSCIENCE, 2024, 39 (01) : 55 - 75
  • [24] Sentence context modifies compound word recognition: Evidence from eye movements
    Juhasz, Barbara J.
    JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY, 2012, 24 (07) : 855 - 870
  • [25] A paradox of apparent brainless behavior: The time-course of compound word recognition
    Schmidtke, Daniel
    Kuperman, Victor
    CORTEX, 2019, 116 : 250 - 267
  • [26] Word-formation of compound nouns with a subordinate connection in the Erzya and Hungarian languages
    Ariskina, T. P.
    VESTNIK UGROVEDENIYA-BULLETIN OF UGRIC STUDIES, 2021, 11 (01): : 7 - 15
  • [27] Word-superiority effect as a function of semantic transparency of Chinese bimorphemic compound words
    Mok, Leh Woon
    LANGUAGE AND COGNITIVE PROCESSES, 2009, 24 (7-8): : 1039 - 1081
  • [28] COMPOUNDING MATTERS: THE P1 AS AN INDEX OF SEMANTIC ACCESS TO COMPOUND WORDS
    Davis, Charles P.
    Libben, Gary
    Segalowitz, Sidney J.
    PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY, 2014, 51 : S34 - S34
  • [29] The influence of nominal prefixes on the formation of compound words in Xitsonga
    Mlambo, Respect
    Matfunjwa, Muzi
    LITERATOR-JOURNAL OF LITERARY CRITICISM COMPARATIVE LINGUISTICS AND LITERARY STUDIES, 2025, 46 (01):
  • [30] The temporal process of visual word recognition of Chinese compound: Behavioral and ERP evidences based on homographic morphemes
    Wu Jianshe
    Chang Jiabao
    Qiu Yinchen
    Dien, Joseph
    ACTA PSYCHOLOGICA SINICA, 2020, 52 (02) : 113 - 127