Time-on-task effect in pseudoneglect

被引:0
|
作者
André Dufour
Pascale Touzalin
Victor Candas
机构
[1] UPS 858 CNRS,Centre d’Etudes de Physiologie Appliquée
来源
Experimental Brain Research | 2007年 / 176卷
关键词
Psychometric Function; Rightward Shift; Bisection Task; Leftward Bias; Transector Location;
D O I
暂无
中图分类号
学科分类号
摘要
Neurologically normal subjects systematically misbisect space during visual line-bisection or similar tasks, generally erring to the left of the veridical center when bisecting horizontal lines, a phenomenon referred to as pseudoneglect. This phenomenon is usually interpreted as enhanced attention toward the left hemispace resulting in an overestimation of the leftward extent of a line. While most studies have examined the role of attention in spatial bias using spatial cueing methods in bisection tasks, Manly et al. (Neuropsychologia 43(12):1721–1728, 2005) proposed an original paradigm in which the participants’ alertness was diminished by sleep deprivation or prolonged execution of a line-bisection task. The authors reported a significant rightward shift in attention related to declining alertness, but they did not control eye movements and, consequently, modifications of scanning and fixation strategies with fatigue cannot be ruled out in their study. Here we examine whether a diminution in alertness induced by a 60-min-long Landmark task would diminish (or even reverse) this attentional bias, when eye movements are absent. Participants performed a forced-choice judgment about the location of a transaction mark in relation to the veridical center of a horizontal line. The results confirmed a significant decrease in the leftward bias over the course of the session but, in contrast to the findings of Manly et al. (2005), we did not observe a reverse bias from the left to the right hemispace. The results are discussed within the context of the hemisphere-activation model.
引用
收藏
页码:532 / 537
页数:5
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [31] Time-on-task mediates the conscientiousness-performance relationship
    Biderman, Michael D.
    Nguyen, Nhung T.
    Sebren, Jennifer
    PERSONALITY AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES, 2008, 44 (04) : 887 - 897
  • [32] Neural cascade of conflict processing: Not just time-on-task
    McKay, Cameron C.
    van den Berg, Berry
    Woldorff, Marty G.
    NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA, 2017, 96 : 184 - 191
  • [33] The same-object benefit is influenced by time-on-task
    Csatho, Arpad
    van der Linden, Dimitri
    Darnai, Gergely
    Hopstaken, Jesper F.
    JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY, 2013, 25 (03) : 319 - 327
  • [34] Effects of chewing gum and time-on-task on alertness and attention
    Allen, A. P.
    Smith, A. P.
    NUTRITIONAL NEUROSCIENCE, 2012, 15 (04) : 176 - 185
  • [35] Effect of uninterrupted time-on-task on students' success in Massive Open Online Courses (MOOCs)
    Lee, Youngjin
    COMPUTERS IN HUMAN BEHAVIOR, 2018, 86 : 174 - 180
  • [36] Quality of Learners' Time and Learning Performance Beyond Quantitative Time-on-Task
    Romero, Margarida
    Barbera, Elena
    INTERNATIONAL REVIEW OF RESEARCH IN OPEN AND DISTRIBUTED LEARNING, 2011, 12 (05): : 125 - 137
  • [37] Exploring Error State in "Time-on-Task" Calculations at Scale
    Chandarana, Mohit
    Deitrick, Elise
    PROCEEDINGS OF THE 54TH ACM TECHNICAL SYMPOSIUM ON COMPUTER SCIENCE EDUCATION, VOL 2, SIGCSE 2023, 2023, : 1336 - 1336
  • [38] The asymmetrical influence of increasing time-on-task on attentional disengagement
    Paladini, Rebecca E.
    Diana, Lorenzo
    Nyffeler, Thomas
    Mosimann, Urs P.
    Nef, Tobias
    Muri, Rene M.
    Cazzoli, Dario
    NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA, 2016, 92 : 107 - 114
  • [39] ATTENTION, TIME-ON-TASK AND CHILDRENS LEARNING - RESEARCH OR IDEOLOGY
    MCNAMARA, DR
    JOURNAL OF EDUCATION FOR TEACHING, 1981, 7 (03) : 284 - 297
  • [40] GENERALIZED EFFECTS OF A HIGHLY STRUCTURED TIME-ON-TASK INTERVENTION
    BAILEY, DB
    BENDER, WN
    STUCK, GB
    WYNE, MD
    PSYCHOLOGICAL REPORTS, 1984, 54 (02) : 483 - 490