Efficient learning for the poor: New insights into literacy acquisition for children

被引:20
作者
Abadzi H. [1 ]
机构
[1] Independent Evaluation Group, World Bank, Washington
关键词
Phonological Awareness; Work Memory Capacity; Dyslexia; Reading Speed; Instructional Time;
D O I
10.1007/s11159-008-9102-3
中图分类号
学科分类号
摘要
Reading depends on the speed of visual recognition and capacity of short-term memory. To understand a sentence, the mind must read it fast enough to capture it within the limits of the short-term memory. This means that children must attain a minimum speed of fairly accurate reading to understand a passage. Learning to read involves "tricking" the brain into perceiving groups of letters as coherent words. This is achieved most efficiently by pairing small units consistently with sounds rather than learning entire words. To link the letters with sounds, explicit and extensive practice is needed; the more complex the spelling of a language, the more practice is necessary. However, schools of low-income students often waste instructional time and lack reading resources, so students cannot get sufficient practice to automatize reading and may remain illiterate for years. Lack of reading fluency in the early grades creates inefficiencies that affect the entire educational system. Neurocognitive research on reading points to benchmarks and monitoring indicators. All students should attain reading speeds of 45-60 words per minute by the end of grade 2 and 120-150 words per minute for grades 6-8. © Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2008.
引用
收藏
页码:581 / 604
页数:23
相关论文
共 58 条
  • [21] Dempster F., The spacing effect: A case study in the failure to apply the results of psychological research, American Psychologist, 43, pp. 627-634, (1988)
  • [22] Evaluación de la velocidad lectora oral y análisis de la correlación de esta variable con la nota global de junio, (2003)
  • [23] Gathercole S.E., Pickering S.J., Knight C., Stegmann Z., Working memory skills and educational attainment: Evidence from national curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age, Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, pp. 1-16, (2004)
  • [24] Hagtvet B., Lyster S.A.H., Literacy Teaching in Scandinavia: Focus on Norway, The Encyclopedia of Language and Education, pp. 225-234, (1998)
  • [25] Hagtvet B., Lyster S.A.H., Spelling Errors of Good and Poor Decoders In:, Dyslexia in Different Languages: Cross-Linguistic Comparisons, pp. 181-207, (2003)
  • [26] Hagtvet B.E., Helland T., Lyster S.A.H., Literacy Acquisition in Norwegian, Handbook of Orthography and Literacy Mahwah, NJ:, (2005)
  • [27] Hanushek E.A., Woessmann L., The Role of Education Quality in Economic Growth, (2007)
  • [28] Harris M., Hatano G., Introduction: A Cross-Linguistic Perspective on Learning to Read and Write, Learning to Read and Write: A Cross-Linguistic Perspective Cambridge,, (1999)
  • [29] Hartley M., Swanson J., Eric V., Retention of Basic Skills Among Dropouts from Egyptian Primary Schools, (1986)
  • [30] Hasbrouck J., Tindal G.A., Oral Reading Fluency Norms: A Valuable Assessment Tool for Reading Teachers, The Reading Teacher, 59, pp. 636-644, (2006)