Grasping performance depends upon the richness of hand feedback

被引:0
作者
Prajith Sivakumar
Derek J. Quinlan
Kevin M. Stubbs
Jody C. Culham
机构
[1] University of Western Ontario,Department of Biology
[2] University of Western Ontario,Brain and Mind Institute
[3] University of Western Ontario,BrainsCAN
[4] Huron University College,Department of Psychology
[5] University of Western Ontario,Department of Psychology
来源
Experimental Brain Research | 2021年 / 239卷
关键词
Grasping; Hand; Vision; Feedback;
D O I
暂无
中图分类号
学科分类号
摘要
Although visual feedback of the hand allows fast and accurate grasping actions, little is known about whether the nature of feedback of the hand affects performance. We investigated kinematics during precision grasping (with the index finger and thumb) when participants received different levels of hand feedback, with or without visual feedback of the target. Specifically, we compared performance when participants saw (1) no hand feedback; (2) only the two critical points on the index finger and thumb tips; (3) 21 points on all digit tips and hand joints; (4) 21 points connected by a “skeleton”, or (5) full feedback of the hand wearing a glove. When less hand feedback was available, participants took longer to execute the movement because they allowed more time to slow the reach and close the hand. When target feedback was unavailable, participants took longer to plan the movement and reached with higher velocity. We were particularly interested in investigating maximum grip aperture (MGA), which can reflect the margin of error that participants allow to compensate for uncertainty. A trend suggested that MGA was smallest when ample feedback was available (skeleton and full hand feedback, regardless of target feedback) and when only essential information about hand and target was provided (2-point hand feedback + target feedback) but increased when non-essential points were included (21-point feedback). These results suggest that visual feedback of the hand affects grasping performance and that, while more feedback is usually beneficial, this is not necessarily always the case.
引用
收藏
页码:835 / 846
页数:11
相关论文
共 163 条
[1]  
Adamovich SV(2009)A virtual reality-based system integrated with fmri to study neural mechanisms of action observation-execution: a proof of concept study Restor Neurol Neurosci 27 209-223
[2]  
August K(2017)When perception trips action! The increase in the perceived size of both hand and target matters in reaching and grasping movements Acta Psychol (Amst) 180 160-168
[3]  
Merians A(2004)Extrastriate body area in human occipital cortex responds to the performance of motor actions Nat Neurosci 7 542-548
[4]  
Tunik E(1996)Visual information and object size in the control of reaching J Mot Behav 28 187-197
[5]  
Ambron E(1996)Specific involvement of human parietal systems in the perception of biological motion and the amygdala J Neurosci 16 3737-3744
[6]  
Schettino LF(2012)Closely overlapping responses to tools and hands in left lateral occipitotemporal cortex J Neurophysiol 107 1443-1456
[7]  
Coyle M(2010)Dissociable neural responses to hands and non-hand body parts in human left extrastriate visual cortex J Neurophysiol 103 3389-3397
[8]  
Astafiev SV(2016)Virtual hand feedback reduces reaction time in an interactive finger reaching task PLoS ONE 11 e0154807-1030
[9]  
Stanley CM(2020)The balance of rigor and reality in developmental neuroscience Neuroimage 7 1019-477
[10]  
Shulman GL(1981)Processing visual feedback information for movement control J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 94 471-89