“Excellence R Us”: university research and the fetishisation of excellence

被引:100
作者
Moore S. [1 ]
Neylon C. [2 ]
Paul Eve M. [3 ]
Paul O’Donnell D. [4 ]
Pattinson D. [5 ]
机构
[1] Kings College, London
[2] Curtin University, Perth
[3] Birkbeck, University of London
[4] Research Square, London
基金
美国安德鲁·梅隆基金会;
关键词
D O I
10.1057/palcomms.2016.105
中图分类号
学科分类号
摘要
The rhetoric of “excellence” is pervasive across the academy. It is used to refer to research outputs as well as researchers, theory and education, individuals and organizations, from art history to zoology. But does “excellence” actually mean anything? Does this pervasive narrative of “excellence” do any good? Drawing on a range of sources we interrogate “excellence” as a concept and find that it has no intrinsic meaning in academia. Rather it functions as a linguistic interchange mechanism. To investigate whether this linguistic function is useful we examine how the rhetoric of excellence combines with narratives of scarcity and competition to show that the hyper-competition that arises from the performance of “excellence” is completely at odds with the qualities of good research. We trace the roots of issues in reproducibility, fraud, and homophily to this rhetoric. But we also show that this rhetoric is an internal, and not primarily an external, imposition. We conclude by proposing an alternative rhetoric based on soundness and capacity-building. In the final analysis, it turns out that that “excellence” is not excellent. Used in its current unqualified form it is a pernicious and dangerous rhetoric that undermines the very foundations of good research and scholarship. This article is published as part of a collection on the future of research assessment. © 2017, The Author(s).
引用
收藏
相关论文
共 150 条
[11]  
Bissell M., Reproducibility: The risks of the replication drive, Nature, 503, 7476, pp. 333-334, (2013)
[12]  
Brembs B., The cost of the rejection-resubmission cycle, The Winnower, (2015)
[13]  
Brembs B., Button K., Munafo M., Deep impact: Unintended consequences of journal rank, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, (2013)
[14]  
Burman L.E., Reed W.R., Alm J., A call for replication studies, Public Finance Review, 38, 6, pp. 787-793, (2010)
[15]  
Calcagno V., Demoinet E., Gollner K., Guidi L., Ruths D., de Mazancourt C., Flows of research manuscripts among scientific journals reveal hidden submission patterns, Science, 338, 6110, pp. 1065-1069, (2012)
[16]  
Campanario J.M., Consolation for the scientist: Sometimes it is hard to publish papers that are later highly cited, Social Studies of Science, 23, pp. 342-362, (1993)
[17]  
Campanario J.M., Commentary on influential books and journal articles initially rejected because of negative referees’ evaluations, Science Communication, 16, 3, pp. 304-325, (1995)
[18]  
Campanario J.M., Have referees rejected some of the most-cited articles of all times?, Journal of the American Society for Information Science, 47, 4, pp. 302-310, (1996)
[19]  
Campanario J.M., Rejecting and resisting Nobel class discoveries: Accounts by Nobel Laureates, Scientometrics, 81, 2, pp. 549-565, (2009)
[20]  
Campanario J.M., Acedo E., Rejecting highly cited papers: The views of scientists who encounter resistance to their discoveries from other scientists, Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 58, pp. 734-743, (2007)