Children’s Reports of Disclosure Recipient Reactions in Forensic Interviews: Comparing the NICHD and MoGP Protocols

被引:15
作者
Ahern E.C. [1 ]
Lamb M.E. [1 ]
机构
[1] University of Cambridge, Cambridge
关键词
Children; Disclosure; Forensic interview;
D O I
10.1007/s11896-016-9205-x
中图分类号
学科分类号
摘要
Reactions from confidantes who receive children’s abuse disclosures can affect children’s well-being and the likelihood that they will recant. Disclosure recipient (DR) reactions were coded in 95 forensic interviews of 4- to 13-year-old alleged sexual abuse victims. Half of the interviews were conducted using the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) Protocol (which includes a disclosure phase focused on the child’s initial abuse report) and the other half using the Memorandum of Good Practice (MoGP), a predecessor of the Achieving Best Evidence (ABE) guidelines used in the UK today (which recommends asking about children’s initial disclosures but has no designated disclosure phase). Children reported a variety of DR reactions, including supportive and unsupportive responses, and noted that many DRs questioned them about the allegations. NICHD interviews contained more references to DR reactions than MoGP interviews. NICHD interviews elicited more DR reaction information using invitations rather than more focused prompts and by asking children explicitly about their disclosures rather than relying on children to provide the information spontaneously. Findings indicated that children may be willing and able to provide disclosure information but may require prompting. © 2016, The Author(s).
引用
收藏
页码:85 / 93
页数:8
相关论文
共 33 条
[11]  
Hershkowitz I., Lanes O., Lamb M.E., Exploring the disclosure of child sexual abuse with alleged victims and their parents, Child Abuse Negl, 23, pp. 175-190, (2007)
[12]  
Johnson M., Magnussen S., Thoresen C., Lonnum K., Burrell L.V., Melinder A., Best practice recommendations still fail to result in action: a national 10-year follow-up study of investigative interviews in CSA cases, Appl Cogn Psychol, 29, pp. 661-668, (2015)
[13]  
Jones D.P., McGraw J.M., Reliable and fictitious accounts of sexual abuse to children, J Interpers Violence, 2, pp. 27-45, (1987)
[14]  
Lamb M.E., Orbach Y., Sternberg K.J., Aldridge J., Pearson S., Stewart H.L., Esplin P.W., Bowler L., Use of a structured investigative protocol enhances the quality of investigative interviews with alleged child sexual abuse in Britain, Appl Cogn Psychol, 23, pp. 449-467, (2009)
[15]  
Lamb M.E., Orbach Y., Hershkowitz I., Esplin P.W., Horowitz D., A structured forensic interview protocol improves the quality and informativeness of investigative interviews with children: a review of research using the NICHD investigative interview protocol, Child Abuse Negl, 31, pp. 1201-1231, (2007)
[16]  
La Rooy D., Katz C., Malloy L.C., Lamb M.E., Do we need to rethink guidance on repeated interviews?, Psychol Public Policy Law, 16, pp. 373-392, (2010)
[17]  
Lawson L., Chaffin M., False negatives in sexual abuse disclosure interviews: incidence and influence of caretaker’s belief in abuse cases of accidental discovery and diagnosis of STD, J Interpers Violence, 7, pp. 532-542, (1992)
[18]  
Lawson M., London K., Tell me everything you discussed: children’s memory for dyadic conversations after a 1-week or a 3-week delay, Behav Sci Law, (2015)
[19]  
Leifer M., Shapiro J.P., Kassem L., The impact of maternal history and behaviour upon foster placement and adjustment in sexually abused girls, Child Abuse Negl, 17, pp. 755-766, (1993)
[20]  
Lyon T.D., Scurich N., Choi K., Handmaker S., Blank K., How did you feel?”: increasing child sexual abuse witnesses’ production of evaluative information, Law Hum Behav, 36, pp. 448-457, (2012)