Spatiotemporal Characteristics of 360-Degree Basic Attention

被引:5
作者
Harada, Yuki [1 ]
Ohyama, Junji [1 ]
机构
[1] Natl Inst Adv Ind Sci & Technol, Human Augmentat Res Ctr, Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan
基金
日本学术振兴会;
关键词
FIELD-OF-VIEW; LINE BISECTION; EYE-MOVEMENTS; PERFORMANCE; PSEUDONEGLECT; METAANALYSIS; NEGLECT; CAPTURE;
D O I
10.1038/s41598-019-52313-3
中图分类号
O [数理科学和化学]; P [天文学、地球科学]; Q [生物科学]; N [自然科学总论];
学科分类号
07 ; 0710 ; 09 ;
摘要
The spatiotemporal characteristics of basic attention are important for understanding attending behaviours in real-life situations, and they are useful for evaluating the accessibility of visual information. However, although people are encircled by their 360-degree surroundings in real life, no study has addressed the general characteristics of attention to 360-degree surroundings. Here, we conducted an experiment using virtual reality technology to examine the spatiotemporal characteristics of attention in a highly controlled basic visual context consisting of a 360-degree surrounding. We measured response times and gaze patterns during the 360-degree search task and examined the spatial distribution of attention and its temporal variations in a 360-degree environment based on the participants' physical position. Data were collected from both younger adults and older adults to consider age-related differences. The results showed the fundamental spatiotemporal characteristics of 360-degree attention, which can be used as basic criteria to analyse the structure of exogenous effects on attention in complex 360-degree surroundings in real-life situations. For practical purposes, we created spherical criteria maps of 360-degree attention, which are useful for estimating attending behaviours to 360-degree environmental information or for evaluating visual information design in living environments, workspaces, or other real-life contexts.
引用
收藏
页数:11
相关论文
共 37 条
[1]   In-vehicle augmented reality TSR to improve driving safety and enhance the driver's experience [J].
Abdi, Lotfi ;
Meddeb, Aref .
SIGNAL IMAGE AND VIDEO PROCESSING, 2018, 12 (01) :75-82
[2]  
[Anonymous], 2001, ISO IEC GUID 71 GUID
[3]   Sensitivity of clinical and behavioural tests of spatial neglect after right hemisphere stroke [J].
Azouvi, P ;
Samuel, C ;
Louis-Dreyfus, A ;
Bernati, T ;
Bartolomeo, P ;
Beis, JM ;
Chokron, S ;
Leclercq, M ;
Marchal, F ;
Martin, Y ;
de Montety, G ;
Olivier, S ;
Perennou, D ;
Pradat-Diehl, P ;
Prairial, C ;
Rode, G ;
Siéroff, E ;
Wiart, L ;
Rousseaux, M .
JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY NEUROSURGERY AND PSYCHIATRY, 2002, 73 (02) :160-166
[4]  
Bacca J, 2014, EDUC TECHNOL SOC, V17, P133
[5]   On the neural origin of pseudoneglect: EEG-correlates of shifts in line bisection performance with manipulation of line length [J].
Benwell, Christopher S. Y. ;
Harvey, Monika ;
Thut, Gregor .
NEUROIMAGE, 2014, 86 :370-380
[6]   Cross-modal attention enhances perceived contrast [J].
Carrasco, Marisa .
PROCEEDINGS OF THE NATIONAL ACADEMY OF SCIENCES OF THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA, 2009, 106 (52) :22039-22040
[7]   Prolonged Disengagement From Attentional Capture in Normal Aging [J].
Cashdollar, Nathan ;
Fukuda, Keisuke ;
Bocklage, Angelika ;
Aurtenetxe, Sara ;
Vogel, Edward K. ;
Gazzaley, Adam .
PSYCHOLOGY AND AGING, 2013, 28 (01) :77-86
[8]   Contextual cueing: Implicit learning and memory of visual context guides spatial attention [J].
Chun, MM ;
Jian, YH .
COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY, 1998, 36 (01) :28-71
[9]   Cumulative meta-analysis of the relationship between useful field of view and driving performance in older adults: Current and future implications [J].
Clay, OJ ;
Wadley, VG ;
Edwards, JD ;
Roth, DL ;
Roenker, DL ;
Ball, KK .
OPTOMETRY AND VISION SCIENCE, 2005, 82 (08) :724-731
[10]  
Crawford C, 1998, HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY, P3