A comparison of two methods of active learning in physics: inventing a general solution versus compare and contrast

被引:28
作者
Chin, Doris B. [1 ]
Chi, Min [2 ]
Schwartz, Daniel L. [1 ]
机构
[1] Stanford Univ, Grad Sch Educ, 485 Lasuen Mall, Stanford, CA 94305 USA
[2] N Carolina State Univ, Dept Comp Sci, 890 Oval Dr,Campus Box 8206, Raleigh, NC 27695 USA
基金
美国国家科学基金会;
关键词
Science education; Science instruction; Inventing; Compare and contrast; Contrasting cases; ANALOGICAL TRANSFER; EXPLANATION; INDUCTION; KNOWLEDGE; FRAMEWORK;
D O I
10.1007/s11251-016-9374-0
中图分类号
G40 [教育学];
学科分类号
040101 ; 120403 ;
摘要
A common approach for introducing students to a new science concept is to present them with multiple cases of the phenomenon and ask them to explore. The expectation is that students will naturally take advantage of the multiple cases to support their learning and seek an underlying principle for the phenomenon. However, the success of such tasks depends not only on the structure of the cases, but also the task that students receive for working with the examples. Two studies used contrasting cases in the context of teaching middle-school students about projectile motion. Using a simulation and the same set of cases for all students, students completed a traditional "compare and contrast" approach, or an instructional method called "inventing," where students try to produce a single general explanation. The results show that inventing led to superior learning. Examination of student worksheets revealed that the "compare and contrast" instruction led students to focus mostly on the level of discrete, surface features of the phenomenon. Rather than trying to account for the variation across cases, students simply noticed each instance of it. In contrast, the inventing task led students to consider how the variations across the cases were related. As a result, "invent" students were more likely to search for and find the unifying functional relation. Driving towards an overall explanation is a fundamental tenet of science, and therefore, it is worthwhile to teach students to do the same.
引用
收藏
页码:177 / 195
页数:19
相关论文
共 33 条
[1]   Learning Through Case Comparisons: A Meta-Analytic Review [J].
Alfieri, Louis ;
Nokes-Malach, Timothy J. ;
Schunn, Christian D. .
EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGIST, 2013, 48 (02) :87-113
[2]   SEXING DAY-OLD CHICKS - A CASE-STUDY AND EXPERT SYSTEMS-ANALYSIS OF A DIFFICULT PERCEPTUAL-LEARNING TASK [J].
BIEDERMAN, I ;
SHIFFRAR, MM .
JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY-LEARNING MEMORY AND COGNITION, 1987, 13 (04) :640-645
[3]  
Bonwell James., 1991, Active Learning: Creating Excitement in the Classroom AEHE-ER1C Higher Education Report No. 1
[4]  
Bransford J. D., 1999, LEARN MIND BRAIN EXP
[5]   NEW APPROACHES TO INSTRUCTION - BECAUSE WISDOM CANT BE TOLD [J].
BRANSFORD, JD ;
FRANKS, JJ ;
VYE, NJ ;
SHERWOOD, RD .
SIMILARITY AND ANALOGICAL REASONING, 1989, :470-497
[6]   Active-Constructive-Interactive: A Conceptual Framework for Differentiating Learning Activities [J].
Chi, Michelene T. H. .
TOPICS IN COGNITIVE SCIENCE, 2009, 1 (01) :73-105
[7]   CATEGORIZATION AND REPRESENTATION OF PHYSICS PROBLEMS BY EXPERTS AND NOVICES [J].
CHI, MTH ;
FELTOVICH, PJ ;
GLASER, R .
COGNITIVE SCIENCE, 1981, 5 (02) :121-152
[8]   Where Hypotheses Come From: Learning New Relations by Structural Alignment [J].
Christie, Stella ;
Gentner, Dedre .
JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT, 2010, 11 (03) :356-373
[9]  
Eisner E. W., 1972, ED ARTISTIC VISION
[10]  
Finn JD, 2012, HANDBOOK OF RESEARCH ON STUDENT ENGAGEMENT, P97, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_5