Five-Year-Old Children's Working Memory Can Be Improved When Children Act On A Transparent Goal Cue

被引:15
作者
Fitamen, Christophe [1 ,2 ,3 ]
Blaye, Agnes [2 ,3 ]
Camos, Valerie [1 ]
机构
[1] Univ Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland
[2] Univ Aix Marseille, Marseille, France
[3] CNRS LPC, Marseille, France
基金
瑞士国家科学基金会;
关键词
EXECUTIVE FUNCTION; NEGLECT; SET; ATTENTION; CAPACITY; DEVELOPS; PICTURES; SWITCH; NORMS;
D O I
10.1038/s41598-019-51869-4
中图分类号
O [数理科学和化学]; P [天文学、地球科学]; Q [生物科学]; N [自然科学总论];
学科分类号
07 ; 0710 ; 09 ;
摘要
Working memory is a key component of human cognition and its development throughout childhood a major predictor of cognitive development and school achievement. Noticeably, preschoolers exhibit poor performance in working memory tasks. The present study aimed at testing different means to improve working memory performance in preschoolers. To this aim, we tested the effect of abstract and transparent goal cues in a Brown-Peterson task performed by 4- and 5-year-old preschoolers. If the transparent goal cue helps to better maintain the instructions, it should lead to better memory performance. Moreover, preschoolers had to track, either visually or with their fingers, the goal cue during the retention interval. If the motor activity favors the active engagement of the children in the task, the finger tracking should lead to improvement in memory performance. Our findings were that 5-year-old children benefitted from a transparent goal cue when they acted on it, while 4-year-old children did not show any improvement. These results suggest that working memory performance can be improved in 5-year-old children when the task embeds elements that can scaffold the task goal.
引用
收藏
页数:8
相关论文
共 28 条
[11]   Setting Goals to Switch Between Tasks: Effect of Cue Transparency on Children's Cognitive Flexibility [J].
Chevalier, Nicolas ;
Blaye, Agnes .
DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2009, 45 (03) :782-797
[12]   Gestures, but not meaningless movements, lighten working memory load when explaining math [J].
Cook, Susan Wagner ;
Yip, Terina Kuangyi ;
Goldin-Meadow, Susan .
LANGUAGE AND COGNITIVE PROCESSES, 2012, 27 (04) :594-610
[13]   Intelligence and the frontal lobe: The organization of goal-directed behavior [J].
Duncan, J ;
Emslie, H ;
Williams, P ;
Johnson, R ;
Freer, C .
COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY, 1996, 30 (03) :257-303
[14]  
Gathercole S., 2006, Working memory and education, DOI DOI 10.1016/B978-012554465-8/50010-7
[15]  
Goldin-Meadow S., 2011, Oxford Handbooks Online, DOI DOI 10.1093/OXFORDHB/9780199541119.013.0057
[16]   DEVELOPMENT OF VOLUNTARY MEMORY IN PRESCHOOL-AGE CHILDREN [J].
ISTOMINA, ZM .
SOVIET PSYCHOLOGY, 1975, 13 (04) :5-64
[17]   Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference [J].
Kane, MJ ;
Engle, RW .
JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY-GENERAL, 2003, 132 (01) :47-70
[18]   Use it or lose it: examining preschoolers' difficulty in maintaining and executing a goal [J].
Marcovitch, Stuart ;
Boseovski, Janet J. ;
Knapp, Robin J. .
DEVELOPMENTAL SCIENCE, 2007, 10 (05) :559-564
[19]   Goal Neglect and Working Memory Capacity in 4-to 6-Year-Old Children [J].
Marcovitch, Stuart ;
Boseovski, Janet J. ;
Kane, Michael J. ;
Knapp, Robin J. .
CHILD DEVELOPMENT, 2010, 81 (06) :1687-1695
[20]   Inner speech as a retrieval aid for task goals: the effects of cue type and articulatory suppression in the random task cuing paradigm [J].
Miyake, A ;
Emerson, MJ ;
Padilla, F ;
Ahn, JC .
ACTA PSYCHOLOGICA, 2004, 115 (2-3) :123-142