Children and Adults Prefer the Egocentric Representation to the Allocentric Representation

被引:7
|
作者
Hu, Qingfen [1 ]
Yang, Ying [1 ]
Huang, Zhenzhen [1 ]
Shao, Yi [2 ]
机构
[1] Beijing Normal Univ, Inst Dev Psychol, Beijing, Peoples R China
[2] Oklahoma City Univ, Dept Psychol, Oklahoma City, OK USA
来源
FRONTIERS IN PSYCHOLOGY | 2018年 / 9卷
基金
中国国家自然科学基金;
关键词
egocentric representation; allocentric representation; strategy preference; children; adults; SPATIAL REPRESENTATION; GEOMETRIC MODULE; ORIENTATION; REORIENTATION; INFANCY; SEARCH; MEMORY; PLACE;
D O I
10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01522
中图分类号
B84 [心理学];
学科分类号
04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
We studied the strategy preference of using the egocentric or the allocentric representation in individuals who have acquired the ability to use both representations. Fifty-seven children aged 5-7 years and 53 adults retrieved toys hidden in one of four identical containers in a square room. We varied the type of spatial representation available in four conditions: (1) only self-motion information (egocentric representation); (2) only external landmark cues (allocentric representation); (3) both self-motion and landmark cues (dual representation); (4) self-motion and landmark cues in conflict (conflict trial). We found that, compared with the allocentric representation, the egocentric representation approached maturity earlier in development and was exploited better in early years. More importantly, in the conflict trials, while both children and adults relied more on egocentric representation, still a small portion of participants chose allocentric representation, especially in the adult group. These results provided evidence that egocentric representation is generally preferred more in both young children and adults.
引用
收藏
页数:7
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [11] Spatial memory in young adults: Gender differences in egocentric and allocentric performance
    Fernandez-Baizan, C.
    Arias, J. L.
    Mendez, M.
    BEHAVIOURAL BRAIN RESEARCH, 2019, 359 : 694 - 700
  • [12] Developmental time course of the acquisition of sequential egocentric and allocentric navigation strategies
    Bullens, Jessie
    Igloi, Kinga
    Berthoz, Alain
    Postma, Albert
    Rondi-Reig, Laure
    JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL CHILD PSYCHOLOGY, 2010, 107 (03) : 337 - 350
  • [13] Neuronal representation of environmental boundaries in egocentric coordinates
    Hinman, James R.
    Chapman, G. William
    Hasselmo, Michael E.
    NATURE COMMUNICATIONS, 2019, 10 (1)
  • [14] Congenital blindness limits allocentric to egocentric switching ability
    Ruggiero, Gennaro
    Ruotolo, Francesco
    Iachini, Tina
    EXPERIMENTAL BRAIN RESEARCH, 2018, 236 (03) : 813 - 820
  • [15] Pointing to and egocentric an allocentric remembered target
    Lemay, M
    Bertram, CP
    Stelmach, GE
    MOTOR CONTROL, 2004, 8 (01) : 16 - 32
  • [16] Egocentric Representation Acquired from Offline Map Learning
    Xiao, Chengli
    Zhang, Lei
    PLOS ONE, 2013, 8 (03):
  • [17] Allocentric but not egocentric visual memory difficulties in adults with ADHD may represent cognitive inefficiency
    Brown, Franklin C.
    Roth, Robert M.
    Katz, Lynda J.
    PSYCHIATRY RESEARCH, 2015, 228 (03) : 649 - 658
  • [18] Egocentric and allocentric search: effects of platform distance and environmental cues
    Tamara, Carolina
    Leffel, Joseph
    Timberlake, William
    ANIMAL COGNITION, 2010, 13 (03) : 565 - 581
  • [19] Representation of morality in children: A qualitative approach
    Maftei, Alexandra
    Holman, Andrei
    JOURNAL OF MORAL EDUCATION, 2020, 49 (02) : 194 - 208
  • [20] Visual information from observing grasping movement in allocentric and egocentric perspectives: development in typical children
    Tinelli, Francesca
    Cioni, Giovanni
    Sandini, Giulio
    Turi, Marco
    Morrone, Maria Concetta
    EXPERIMENTAL BRAIN RESEARCH, 2017, 235 (07) : 2039 - 2047