Two- and 3-year-olds integrate linguistic and pedagogical cues in guiding inductive generalization and exploration

被引:30
作者
Butler, Lucas P. [1 ]
Tomasello, Michael [2 ]
机构
[1] Univ Maryland, Dept Human Dev & Quantitat Methodol, College Pk, MD 20742 USA
[2] Max Planck Inst Evolutionary Anthropol, Dept Dev & Comparat Psychol, D-04103 Leipzig, Germany
关键词
Social learning; Pedagogy; Inductive inference; Generalization; Exploration; Cultural learning; Pragmatic reasoning; INFANTS; OBJECT; CATEGORIES; INFERENCES; APPRECIATION; ARTIFACTS; OTHERS; LEARN; WORD;
D O I
10.1016/j.jecp.2015.12.001
中图分类号
B844 [发展心理学(人类心理学)];
学科分类号
040202 ;
摘要
Young children can in principle make generic inferences (e.g., "doffels are magnetic") on the basis of their own individual experience. Recent evidence, however, shows that by 4 years of age children make strong generic inferences on the basis of a single pedagogical demonstration with an individual (e.g., an adult demonstrates for the child that a single "doffel" is magnetic). In the current experiments, we extended this to look at younger children, investigating how the mechanisms underlying this phenomenon are integrated with other aspects of inductive inference during early development. We found that both 2- and 3-year-olds used pedagogical cues to guide such generic inferences, but only so long as the "doffel" was linguistically labeled. In a follow-up study, 3-year-olds, but not 2-year-olds, continued to make this generic inference even if the word "doffel" was uttered incidentally and non-referentially in a context preceding the pedagogical demonstration, thereby simply marking the opportunity to learn about a culturally important category. By 3 years of age, then, young children show a remarkable ability to flexibly combine different sources of culturally relevant information (e.g., linguistic labeling, pedagogy) to make the kinds of generic inferences so central in human cultural learning. (C) 2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
引用
收藏
页码:64 / 78
页数:15
相关论文
共 51 条
[1]  
[Anonymous], 1989, Categorization and naming in children: Problems of induction
[2]  
[Anonymous], 1987, INTENTIONAL STANCE
[3]  
[Anonymous], 1996, RELEVANCE COMMUNICAT
[4]   INFANTS ABILITY TO DRAW INFERENCES ABOUT NONOBVIOUS OBJECT PROPERTIES - EVIDENCE FROM EXPLORATORY PLAY [J].
BALDWIN, DA ;
MARKMAN, EM ;
MELARTIN, RL .
CHILD DEVELOPMENT, 1993, 64 (03) :711-728
[5]   Accessing the unsaid: The role of scalar alternatives in children's pragmatic inference [J].
Barner, David ;
Brooks, Neon ;
Bale, Alan .
COGNITION, 2011, 118 (01) :84-93
[6]   CONTROLLING THE FALSE DISCOVERY RATE - A PRACTICAL AND POWERFUL APPROACH TO MULTIPLE TESTING [J].
BENJAMINI, Y ;
HOCHBERG, Y .
JOURNAL OF THE ROYAL STATISTICAL SOCIETY SERIES B-STATISTICAL METHODOLOGY, 1995, 57 (01) :289-300
[7]   Discovering the false discovery rate [J].
Benjamini, Yoav .
JOURNAL OF THE ROYAL STATISTICAL SOCIETY SERIES B-STATISTICAL METHODOLOGY, 2010, 72 :405-416
[8]   Intention, history, and artifact concepts [J].
Bloom, P .
COGNITION, 1996, 60 (01) :1-29
[9]   The double-edged sword of pedagogy: Instruction limits spontaneous exploration and discovery [J].
Bonawitz, Elizabeth ;
Shafto, Patrick ;
Gweon, Hyowon ;
Goodman, Noah D. ;
Spelke, Elizabeth ;
Schulz, Laura .
COGNITION, 2011, 120 (03) :322-330
[10]   Object names and object functions serve as cues to categories for infants [J].
Booth, AE ;
Waxman, S .
DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2002, 38 (06) :948-957