Problem behavior in young children referred with language difficulties: Relations to language and intentional communication

被引:5
作者
Jansen, Rianne [1 ]
Maljaars, Jarymke [1 ]
Verhappen, Anouk [1 ]
Zink, Inge [1 ,2 ]
Steyaert, Jean [1 ]
Noens, Ilse [1 ]
机构
[1] Katholieke Univ Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
[2] UZ Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
关键词
Language difficulties; intentional communication; problem behavior; early childhood; PRAGMATIC LANGUAGE; BEHAVIORAL/EMOTIONAL PROBLEMS; CHALLENGING BEHAVIOR; PRESCHOOL-CHILDREN; SCHOOL-CHILDREN; EARLY-CHILDHOOD; AUTISM; SKILLS; DELAY; PSYCHOPATHOLOGY;
D O I
10.1177/2396941519900076
中图分类号
G76 [特殊教育];
学科分类号
040109 ;
摘要
Background and aims: This exploratory study aims to examine the relative contribution of language and intentional communication to internalizing and externalizing problem behavior. Methods: Twenty-nine Dutch-speaking children (age range 24-46 months) referred with language difficulties participated in this study. For the majority of children, these early language difficulties appeared to be part of a broader neurodevelopmental disorder, mainly autism spectrum disorder. Parent ratings on the Achenbach Child Behavior Checklist 1 1/2-5 were predicted from children's language level and intentional communicative abilities, the latter being assessed by both parent report and direct observation. In all series of hierarchical regression analyses, chronological age and nonverbal mental age were included as covariates. Results: Parents commonly reported withdrawal, emotionally reactive behavior, attention problems, and aggressive behavior. Parent-rated intentional communication was the most important predictor of internalizing problem behavior and played an important role in the prediction of aggressive behavior as well. However, chronological age and/or nonverbal mental age also predicted parent-rated levels of externalizing problem behavior, especially attention problems. Conclusions: The relation between language difficulties and problem behavior may be influenced by maturation and children's ability to communicate intentionally. Implications: Language proficiency should, therefore, be independently assessed from children's intentional communicative abilities which, in turn, may differ across contexts.
引用
收藏
页数:16
相关论文
共 91 条
[81]  
Tellegen P. J., 2013, SNIJDERS OOMEN NIETV
[82]  
van der Meulen B.F., 2002, Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID) II. Nederlandse versie
[83]   Relationships Between Child Emotional and Behavioral Symptoms and Caregiver Strain and Parenting Stress [J].
Vaughan, Ellen L. ;
Feinn, Richard ;
Bernard, Stanley ;
Brereton, Maria ;
Kaufman, Joy S. .
JOURNAL OF FAMILY ISSUES, 2013, 34 (04) :534-556
[84]   Measuring Pragmatic Language in Speakers With Autism Spectrum Disorders: Comparing the Children's Communication Checklist-2 and the Test of Pragmatic Language [J].
Volden, Joanne ;
Phillips, Linda .
AMERICAN JOURNAL OF SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY, 2010, 19 (03) :204-212
[85]   Effects of Augmentative and Alternative Communication on Challenging Behavior: A Meta-Analysis [J].
Walker, Virginia L. ;
Snell, Martha E. .
AUGMENTATIVE AND ALTERNATIVE COMMUNICATION, 2013, 29 (02) :117-131
[86]  
Wetherby A.M., 1993, COMMUNICATION SYMBOL
[87]  
Wetherby A. M., 2002, COMMUNICATION SYMBOL
[88]   Emotional and behavioural outcomes later in childhood and adolescence for children with specific language impairments: meta-analyses of controlled prospective studies [J].
Yew, Shaun Goh Kok ;
O'Kearney, Richard .
JOURNAL OF CHILD PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY, 2013, 54 (05) :516-524
[89]  
Zink I., 2003, N CDIS KORTE VORMEN
[90]  
Zink I., 2007, N CDI 3