Seeing Minds in Others - Can Agents with Robotic Appearance Have Human-Like Preferences?

被引:71
作者
Martini, Molly C. [1 ]
Gonzalez, Christian A. [1 ]
Wiese, Eva [1 ]
机构
[1] George Mason Univ, Dept Psychol, Fairfax, VA 22030 USA
关键词
ATTRIBUTION; PERCEPTION; BIAS;
D O I
10.1371/journal.pone.0146310
中图分类号
O [数理科学和化学]; P [天文学、地球科学]; Q [生物科学]; N [自然科学总论];
学科分类号
07 ; 0710 ; 09 ;
摘要
Ascribing mental states to non-human agents has been shown to increase their likeability and lead to better joint-task performance in human-robot interaction (HRI). However, it is currently unclear what physical features non-human agents need to possess in order to trigger mind attribution and whether different aspects of having a mind (e.g., feeling pain, being able to move) need different levels of human-likeness before they are readily ascribed to non-human agents. The current study addresses this issue by modeling how increasing the degree of human-like appearance (on a spectrum from mechanistic to humanoid to human) changes the likelihood by which mind is attributed towards non-human agents. We also test whether different internal states (e.g., being hungry, being alive) need different degrees of humanness before they are ascribed to non-human agents. The results suggest that the relationship between physical appearance and the degree to which mind is attributed to non-human agents is best described as a two-linear model with no change in mind attribution on the spectrum from mechanistic to humanoid robot, but a significant increase in mind attribution as soon as human features are included in the image. There seems to be a qualitative difference in the perception of mindful versus mindless agents given that increasing human-like appearance alone does not increase mind attribution until a certain threshold is reached, that is: agents need to be classified as having a mind first before the addition of more human-like features significantly increases the degree to which mind is attributed to that agent.
引用
收藏
页数:23
相关论文
共 29 条
[1]   Do triangles play tricks?: Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development [J].
Abell, F ;
Happé, F ;
Frith, U .
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT, 2000, 15 (01) :1-16
[2]   Taking another person's perspective increases self-referential neural processing [J].
Ames, Daniel L. ;
Jenkins, Adrianna C. ;
Banaji, Mahzarin R. ;
Mitchell, Jason P. .
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE, 2008, 19 (07) :642-644
[3]  
Arnhold E., 2014, easynls: Easy nonlinear model
[4]  
Baron-Cohen S., 1997, Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind
[5]   Infant-like social interactions between a robot and a human caregiver. [J].
Breazeal, C ;
Scassellati, B .
ADAPTIVE BEHAVIOR, 2000, 8 (01) :49-74
[6]   Movement and mind:: A functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns [J].
Castelli, F ;
Happé, F ;
Frith, U ;
Frith, C .
NEUROIMAGE, 2000, 12 (03) :314-325
[7]  
Cohen J., 2002, Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, V3rd, DOI 10.4324/9780203774441
[8]   Effect of perspective taking on the cognitive representation of persons: A merging of self and other [J].
Davis, MH ;
Conklin, L ;
Smith, A ;
Luce, C .
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY, 1996, 70 (04) :713-726
[9]  
Dennett D. C., 1987, INTENTIONAL STANCE
[10]   When we need a human: Motivational determinants of anthropomorphism [J].
Epley, Nicholas ;
Waytz, Adam ;
Akalis, Scott ;
Cacioppo, John T. .
SOCIAL COGNITION, 2008, 26 (02) :143-155