Immersive Virtual Reality Avatars for Embodiment Illusions in People With Mild to Borderline Intellectual Disability: User-Centered Development and Feasibility Study

被引:7
作者
Langener, Simon [1 ,2 ,3 ]
Klaassen, Randy [1 ]
VanDerNagel, Joanne [1 ,2 ,3 ]
Heylen, Dirk [1 ]
机构
[1] Univ Twente, Dept Human Media Interact, POB 217, NL-7500 AE Enschede, Netherlands
[2] Tactus Addict Care, Ctr Addict & Intellectual Disabil, Enschede, Netherlands
[3] Radboud Univ Nijmegen, Nijmegen Inst Scientist Practitioners Addict, Nijmegen, Netherlands
关键词
virtual reality; VR; embodiment; avatar; embodied learning; body-centered; intellectual disability; addiction; user-centered design; BODY OWNERSHIP; ILLUSORY OWNERSHIP; SELF-AVATAR; SENSE; AGENCY;
D O I
10.2196/39966
中图分类号
R19 [保健组织与事业(卫生事业管理)];
学科分类号
摘要
Background: Immersive virtual reality (IVR) has been investigated as a tool for treating psychiatric conditions. In particular, the practical nature of IVR, by offering a doing instead of talking approach, could support people who do not benefit from existing treatments. Hence, people with mild to borderline intellectual disability (MBID; IQ=50-85) might profit particularly from IVR therapies, for instance, to circumvent issues in understanding relevant concepts and interrelations. In this context, immersing the user into a virtual body (ie, avatar) appears promising for enhancing learning (eg, by changing perspectives) and usability (eg, natural interactions). However, design requirements, immersion procedures, and proof of concept of such embodiment illusion (ie, substituting the real body with a virtual one) have not been explored in this group. Objective: Our study aimed to establish design guidelines for IVR embodiment illusions in people with MBID. We explored 3 factors to induce the illusion, by testing the avatar's appearance, locomotion using IVR controllers, and virtual object manipulation. Furthermore, we report on the feasibility to induce the embodiment illusion and provide procedural guidance. Methods: We conducted a user-centered study with 29 end users in care facilities, to investigate the avatar's appearance, controller-based locomotion (ie, teleport, joystick, or hybrid), and object manipulation. Overall, 3 iterations were conducted using semistructured interviews to explore design factors to induce embodiment illusions in our group. To further understand the influence of interactions on the illusion, we measured the sense of embodiment (SoE) during 5 interaction tasks. Results: IVR embodiment illusions can be induced in adults with MBID. To induce the illusion, having a high degree of control over the body outweighed avatar customization, despite the participants' desire to replicate their own body image. Similarly, the highest SoE was measured during object manipulation tasks, which required a combination of (virtual) locomotion and object manipulation behavior. Notably, interactions that are implausible (eg, teleport and occlusions when grabbing) showed a negative influence on SoE. In contrast, implementing artificial interaction aids into the IVR avatar's hands (ie, for user interfaces) did not diminish the illusion, presuming that the control was unimpaired. Nonetheless, embodiment illusions showed a tedious and complex need for (control) habituation (eg, motion sickness), possibly hindering uptake in practice. Conclusions: Balancing the embodiment immersion by focusing on interaction habituation (eg, controller-based locomotion) and lowering customization effort seems crucial to achieve both high SoE and usability for people with MBID. Hence, future studies should investigate the requirements for natural IVR avatar interactions by using multisensory integrations for the virtual body (eg, animations, physics-based collision, and touch) and other interaction techniques (eg, hand tracking and redirected walking). In addition, procedures and use for learning should be explored for tailored mental health therapies in people with MBID.
引用
收藏
页数:17
相关论文
共 83 条
[1]  
[Anonymous], 1966, The hidden dimension
[2]  
[Anonymous], 2006, Virtual reality
[3]  
Argelaguet F, 2016, P IEEE VIRT REAL ANN, P3, DOI 10.1109/VR.2016.7504682
[4]   Illusory ownership of a virtual child body causes overestimation of object sizes and implicit attitude changes [J].
Banakou, Domna ;
Groten, Raphaela ;
Slater, Mel .
PROCEEDINGS OF THE NATIONAL ACADEMY OF SCIENCES OF THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA, 2013, 110 (31) :12846-12851
[5]   Full-body illusions and minimal phenomenal selfhood [J].
Blanke, Olaf ;
Metzinger, Thomas .
TRENDS IN COGNITIVE SCIENCES, 2009, 13 (01) :7-13
[6]  
Boletsis Costas, 2017, Multimodal Technologies and Interaction, V1, DOI 10.3390/mti1040024
[7]   The Critical Role of Self-Contact for Embodiment in Virtual Reality [J].
Bovet, Sidney ;
Debarba, Henrique Galvan ;
Herbelin, Bruno ;
Molla, Eray ;
Boulic, Ronan .
IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON VISUALIZATION AND COMPUTER GRAPHICS, 2018, 24 (04) :1428-1436
[8]   VR4VR: Vocational Rehabilitation of Individuals with Disabilities in Immersive Virtual Reality Environments [J].
Bozgeyikli, Lal ;
Bozgeyikli, Evren ;
Clevenger, Matthew ;
Raij, Andrew ;
Alqasemi, Redwan ;
Sundarrao, Stephen ;
Dubey, Rajiv .
8TH ACM INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE ON PERVASIVE TECHNOLOGIES RELATED TO ASSISTIVE ENVIRONMENTS (PETRA 2015), 2015,
[9]  
Braun V., 2006, Qual Res Psychol, V3, P77, DOI [DOI 10.1191/1478088706QP063OA, DOI 10.1080/10875549.2021.1929659, https://doi.org/10.1191/1478088706qp063oa]
[10]  
Brown D., 2006, Virtual Reality, V10, P241, DOI [DOI 10.1007/S10055-006-0042-6, 10.1007/s10055-006-0042-6]