The influence of contextual faces on bilingual language control

被引:31
作者
Liu, Cong [1 ,2 ]
Timmer, Kalinka [3 ]
Jiao, Lu [4 ]
Yuan, Yuan [1 ,2 ]
Wang, Ruiming [1 ,2 ]
机构
[1] South China Normal Univ, Sch Psychol, Guangdong Prov Key Lab Mental Hlth & Cognit Sci, Guangzhou 510631, Guangdong, Peoples R China
[2] South China Normal Univ, Sch Psychol, Ctr Studies Psychol Applicat, Guangzhou 510631, Guangdong, Peoples R China
[3] Univ Pompeu Fabra, CBC, Barcelona, Spain
[4] Beijing Normal Univ, Sch Psychol, Beijing Key Lab Appl Expt Psychol, Beijing, Peoples R China
关键词
Switch costs; reversed language dominant effect; language switching; contextual faces; language production; HIGHLY PROFICIENT BILINGUALS; COGNITIVE CONTROL; SPEECH PRODUCTION; INHIBITION; SELECTION; SWITCH; BRAIN; COSTS; FMRI;
D O I
10.1177/1747021819836713
中图分类号
B84 [心理学];
学科分类号
04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
How do faces with social-cultural identity affect bilingual language control? We approach this question by looking at the switch cost patterns and reversed language dominance effect, which are suggested to reflect bilingual language control mechanisms, in the absence (i.e., baseline context) or presence of faces with socio-cultural identity (Asian or Caucasian). In separate blocks, the face matched (i.e., congruent context) or mismatched (i.e., incongruent context) the language to be spoken. In addition, cue preparation time was manipulated to be long (Experiment 1) or short (Experiment 2). In both experiments, a unique asymmetric switch cost with larger costs for L2 was observed in the congruent context as compared with the baseline and incongruent contexts. Furthermore, the reversed language dominance effect was not modulated across contexts. These results suggest a critical role of contextual faces in modulating local but not global language control. Thus, bilingual language control changes flexibly within an environment that includes faces with socio-cultural identity.
引用
收藏
页码:2313 / 2327
页数:15
相关论文
共 52 条
[41]   Iconic Native Culture Cues Inhibit Second Language Production in a Non-immigrant Population: Evidence from Bengali-English Bilinguals [J].
Roychoudhuri, Kesaban S. ;
Prasad, Seema G. ;
Mishra, Ramesh K. .
FRONTIERS IN PSYCHOLOGY, 2016, 7
[42]   Language switching in bilingual speech production In search of the language-specific selection mechanism [J].
Schwieter, John W. ;
Sunderman, Gretchen .
MENTAL LEXICON, 2008, 3 (02) :214-238
[43]   STANDARDIZED SET OF 260 PICTURES - NORMS FOR NAME AGREEMENT, IMAGE AGREEMENT, FAMILIARITY, AND VISUAL COMPLEXITY [J].
SNODGRASS, JG ;
VANDERWART, M .
JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY-HUMAN LEARNING AND MEMORY, 1980, 6 (02) :174-215
[44]  
Timmer K., 2018, BILING-LANG COGN, P1
[45]  
Timmer K., 2017, Bilingual Processing and Acquisition, V6, P123, DOI [10.1075/bpa.6.06tim, DOI 10.1075/BPA.6.06TIM]
[46]   Earlier and more distributed neural networks for bilinguals than monolinguals during switching [J].
Timmer, Kalinka ;
Grundy, John G. ;
Bialystok, Ellen .
NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA, 2017, 106 :245-260
[47]   Role of inhibition in language switching: Evidence from event-related brain potentials in overt picture naming [J].
Verhoef, Kim ;
Roelofs, Ardi ;
Chwilla, Dorothee J. .
COGNITION, 2009, 110 (01) :84-99
[48]   Cognitive control and word recognition speed influence the Stroop effect in bilinguals [J].
Wang, Ruiming ;
Fan, Xiaoyue ;
Liu, Cong ;
Cai, Zhenguang G. .
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY, 2016, 51 (02) :93-101
[49]   Can Faces Prime a Language? [J].
Woumans, Evy ;
Martin, Clara D. ;
Vanden Bulcke, Charlotte ;
Van Assche, Eva ;
Costa, Albert ;
Hartsuiker, Robert J. ;
Duyck, Wouter .
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE, 2015, 26 (09) :1343-1352
[50]   The influence of short-term language-switching training on the plasticity of the cognitive control mechanism in bilingual word production [J].
Wu, Junjie ;
Kang, Chunyan ;
Ma, Fengyang ;
Gao, Xiaoyi ;
Guo, Taomei .
QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2018, 71 (10) :2115-2128