Defining and measuring physicians' responses to clinical reminders

被引:29
作者
Vashitz, Geva [1 ]
Meyer, Joachim [1 ]
Parmet, Yisrael [1 ]
Peleg, Roni [1 ,2 ]
Goldfarb, Dan [2 ]
Porath, Avi [1 ,3 ]
Gilutz, Harel [1 ,3 ]
机构
[1] Ben Gurion Univ Negev, IL-84105 Beer Sheva, Israel
[2] Clalit Hlth Serv, Beer Sheva, Israel
[3] Soroka Univ, Med Ctr, Beer Sheva, Israel
关键词
Guidelines; Decision aids; Medical decision support systems; Treatment gap; Cholesterol; Dyslipidemia; Compliance; Reliance; Spillover; Reactance; Cognitive engineering; DECISION-SUPPORT-SYSTEMS; PREVENTIVE CARE; INFORMATION-TECHNOLOGY; COMPUTER REMINDERS; PATIENT OUTCOMES; WARNING LABELS; HEART-DISEASE; HEALTH-CARE; ENFORCEMENT; BARRIERS;
D O I
10.1016/j.jbi.2008.10.001
中图分类号
TP39 [计算机的应用];
学科分类号
081203 ; 0835 ;
摘要
Decision-support systems, and specifically rule-based clinical reminders, are becoming common in medical practice. Despite their potential to improve clinical outcomes, physicians do not always use information from these systems. Concepts from the cognitive engineering literature on users' responses to warning systems may help to define physicians' responses to reminders. Based on this literature, we suggest an exhaustive set of possible responses to clinical reminders, consisting of four responses named "Compliance", "Reliance", "Spillover" and "Reactance". We suggest statistical measures to estimate these responses and empirically demonstrate them on data from a large-scale clinical reminder system for secondary prevention of cardiovascular diseases. There was evidence for Compliance, probably since the physicians found the reminders informative, but not for Reliance, in line with the notion that Compliance and Reliance are two distinct types of trust in information from decision-support systems. Our research Supports the notion that CDSS can promote closing the treatment gap and improve physicians' adherence to guidelines. (C) 2008 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
引用
收藏
页码:317 / 326
页数:10
相关论文
共 80 条
[1]  
Agrawal A, 2004, STUD HEALTH TECHNOL, V107, P111
[2]   THE EFFECTIVENESS OF ALCOHOL WARNING LABELS - A REVIEW AND EXTENSION [J].
ANDREWS, JC .
AMERICAN BEHAVIORAL SCIENTIST, 1995, 38 (04) :622-632
[3]  
[Anonymous], 2013, Psychological Reactance: A Theory of Freedom and Control
[4]  
Antman Elliott M., 2004, J Am Coll Cardiol, V44, pE1, DOI 10.1016/j.jacc.2004.07.014
[5]   Some unintended consequences of information technology in health care: The nature of patient care information system-related errors [J].
Ash, JS ;
Berg, M ;
Coiera, E .
JOURNAL OF THE AMERICAN MEDICAL INFORMATICS ASSOCIATION, 2004, 11 (02) :104-112
[6]   The clinical value of computerized information services - A review of 98 randomized clinical trials [J].
Balas, EA ;
Austin, SM ;
Mitchell, JA ;
Ewigman, BG ;
Bopp, KD ;
Brown, GD .
ARCHIVES OF FAMILY MEDICINE, 1996, 5 (05) :271-278
[7]   Self-initiated and respondent actions in a simulated control task [J].
Bitan, Yuval ;
Meyer, Joachim .
ERGONOMICS, 2007, 50 (05) :763-788
[8]  
Bliss JP, 2006, HUM FAC ER, P231
[9]   GLIF3: a representation format for sharable computer-interpretable clinical practice guidelines [J].
Boxwala, AA ;
Peleg, M ;
Tu, S ;
Ogunyemi, O ;
Zeng, QT ;
Wang, DW ;
Patel, VL ;
Greenes, RA ;
Shortliffe, EH .
JOURNAL OF BIOMEDICAL INFORMATICS, 2004, 37 (03) :147-161
[10]   Forbidden fruit versus tainted fruit: Effects of warning labels on attraction to television violence [J].
Bushman, BJ ;
Stack, AD .
JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY-APPLIED, 1996, 2 (03) :207-226