Motion camouflage induced by zebra stripes

被引:56
|
作者
How, Martin J. [1 ]
Zanker, Johannes M. [2 ]
机构
[1] Univ Queensland, Queensland Brain Inst, St Lucia, Qld 4000, Australia
[2] Royal Holloway Univ London, Dept Psychol, Egham TW20 0EX, Surrey, England
关键词
Camouflage; Illusory motion; Aliasing; Aperture effect; Equus burchelli; WAGON-WHEEL-ILLUSION; VISUAL-MOTION; PERCEPTION; PATTERN; TSETSE; SIGNAL; REAL;
D O I
10.1016/j.zool.2013.10.004
中图分类号
Q95 [动物学];
学科分类号
071002 ;
摘要
The functional significance of the zebra coat stripe pattern is one of the oldest questions in evolutionary biology, having troubled scientists ever since Charles Darwin and Alfred Russel Wallace first disagreed on the subject. While different theories have been put forward to address this question, the idea that the stripes act to confuse or 'dazzle' observers remains one of the most plausible. However, the specific mechanisms by which this may operate have not been investigated in detail. In this paper, we investigate how motion of the zebra's high contrast stripes creates visual effects that may act as a form of motion camouflage. We simulated a biologically motivated motion detection algorithm to analyse motion signals generated by different areas on a zebra's body during displacements of their retinal images. Our simulations demonstrate that the motion signals that these coat patterns generate could be a highly misleading source of information. We suggest that the observer's visual system is flooded with erroneous motion signals that correspond to two well-known visual illusions: (i) the wagon-wheel effect (perceived motion inversion due to spatiotemporal aliasing); and (ii) the barber-pole illusion (misperceived direction of motion due to the aperture effect), and predict that these two illusory effects act together to confuse biting insects approaching from the air, or possibly mammalian predators during the hunt, particularly when two or more zebras are observed moving together as a herd. (C) 2013 Elsevier GmbH. All rights reserved.
引用
收藏
页码:163 / 170
页数:8
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [31] Motion-induced position mis-localization predicts the severity of Alzheimer's disease
    Song, Yamin
    Wang, Huiting
    JOURNAL OF NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2020, 14 (02) : 333 - 345
  • [32] Visually induced motion sickness: Single- versus dual-axis motion
    Diels, Cyriel
    Howarth, Peter A.
    DISPLAYS, 2011, 32 (04) : 175 - 180
  • [33] Tilt aftereffects and tilt illusions induced by fast translational motion: Evidence for motion streaks
    Apthorp, Deborah
    Alais, David
    JOURNAL OF VISION, 2009, 9 (01):
  • [34] Illusory position shift induced by plaid motion
    Hisakata, Rumi
    Murakami, Ikuya
    VISION RESEARCH, 2009, 49 (24) : 2902 - 2910
  • [35] Motor-induced visual motion: hand movements driving visual motion perception
    Keetels, Mirjam
    Stekelenburg, Jeroen J.
    EXPERIMENTAL BRAIN RESEARCH, 2014, 232 (09) : 2865 - 2877
  • [36] Motion Extrapolation for Eye Movements Predicts Perceived Motion-Induced Position Shifts
    van Heusden, Elle
    Rolfs, Martin
    Cavanagh, Patrick
    Hogendoorn, Hinze
    JOURNAL OF NEUROSCIENCE, 2018, 38 (38) : 8243 - 8250
  • [37] Motion-form interaction: Motion and form aftereffects induced by distorted static natural scenes
    Rifai, Katharina
    Habtegiorgis, Selam W.
    Erlenwein, Caroline
    Wahl, Siegfried
    JOURNAL OF VISION, 2020, 20 (13): : 1 - 9
  • [38] Vection can be induced in the absence of explicit motion stimuli
    Seno, Takeharu
    Ito, Hiroyuki
    Sunaga, Shoji
    EXPERIMENTAL BRAIN RESEARCH, 2012, 219 (02) : 235 - 244
  • [39] Complexity and specificity of experimentally induced expectations in motion perception
    Nikos Gekas
    Matthew Chalk
    Aaron R Seitz
    Peggy Seriès
    BMC Neuroscience, 14 (Suppl 1)
  • [40] The role of mask coherence in motion-induced blindness
    Wells, Erika T.
    Leber, Andrew B.
    Sparrow, John E.
    PERCEPTION, 2011, 40 (12) : 1503 - 1518