Measurement and Design Heterogeneity in Perceived Message Effectiveness Studies: A Call for Research

被引:18
作者
Noar, Seth M. [1 ,2 ]
Barker, Joshua [1 ]
Yzer, Marco [3 ]
机构
[1] Univ North Carolina Chapel Hill, Sch Media & Journalism, Chapel Hill, NC 27599 USA
[2] Univ North Carolina Chapel Hill, Lineberger Comprehens Canc Ctr, Chapel Hill, NC 27599 USA
[3] Univ Minnesota, Hubbard Sch Journalism & Mass Commun, Minneapolis, MN 55455 USA
关键词
Perceived Effectiveness; Message; Meta-analysis;
D O I
10.1093/joc/jqy047
中图分类号
G2 [信息与知识传播];
学科分类号
05 ; 0503 ;
摘要
Ratings of perceived message effectiveness (PME) are commonly used during message testing and selection, operating under the assumption that messages scoring higher on PME are more likely to affect actual message effectiveness (AME)—for instance, intentions and behaviors. Such a practice has clear utility, particularly when selecting from a large pool of messages. Recently, O’Keefe (2018) argued against the validity of PME as a basis for message selection. He conducted a meta-analysis of mean ratings of PME and AME, testing how often two messages that differ on PME similarly differ on AME, as tested in separate samples. Comparing 151 message pairs derived from 35 studies, he found that use of PME would only result in choosing a more effective message 58% of the time, which is little better than chance. On that basis, O’Keefe concluded that “message designers might dispense with questions about expected or perceived persuasiveness (PME), and instead pretest messages for actual effectiveness” (p. 135). We do not believe that the meta-analysis supports this conclusion, given the measurement and design issues in the set of studies O’Keefe analyzed. © The Author(s) 2018. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of International Communication Association. All rights reserved.
引用
收藏
页码:990 / 993
页数:4
相关论文
共 7 条
[1]  
[Anonymous], PREDICTING AND CHANG
[2]   Meta-analysis of the relationship between risk perception and health behavior: The example of vaccination [J].
Brewer, Noel T. ;
Chapman, Gretchen B. ;
Gibbons, Frederick X. ;
Gerrard, Meg ;
McCaul, Kevin D. ;
Weinstein, Neil D. .
HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY, 2007, 26 (02) :136-145
[3]   The perceived effectiveness of persuasive messages: Questions of structure, referent, and bias [J].
Dillard, James Price ;
Ye, Sun .
JOURNAL OF HEALTH COMMUNICATION, 2008, 13 (02) :149-168
[4]   Perceived Message Effectiveness Measures in Tobacco Education Campaigns: A Systematic Review [J].
Noar, Seth M. ;
Bell, Trevor ;
Kelley, Dannielle ;
Barker, Joshua ;
Yzer, Marco .
COMMUNICATION METHODS AND MEASURES, 2018, 12 (04) :295-313
[5]   Message Pretesting Using Assessments of Expected or Perceived Persuasiveness: Evidence About Diagnosticity of Relative Actual Persuasiveness [J].
O'Keefe, Daniel J. .
JOURNAL OF COMMUNICATION, 2018, 68 (01) :120-142
[6]   The effect of cancer warning statements on alcohol consumption intentions [J].
Pettigrew, Simone ;
Jongenelis, Michelle I. ;
Glance, David ;
Chikritzhs, Tanya ;
Pratt, Iain S. ;
Slevin, Terry ;
Liang, Wenbin ;
Wakefield, Melanie .
HEALTH EDUCATION RESEARCH, 2016, 31 (01) :60-69
[7]   On the Conceptual Ambiguity Surrounding Perceived Message Effectiveness [J].
Yzer, Marco ;
LoRusso, Susan ;
Nagler, Rebekah H. .
HEALTH COMMUNICATION, 2015, 30 (02) :125-134