Short run reference points and long run performance. (No) Evidence from running data

被引:6
作者
Soetevent, Adriaan R. [1 ]
机构
[1] Univ Groningen, Fac Econ & Business, Nettelbosje 2, NL-9747 AE Groningen, Netherlands
关键词
Goal setting; Bunching; Reference points; Running; REFERENCE-DEPENDENT PREFERENCES; SELF-CONTROL; GOALS; MOTIVATION; EXPERIENCE; ECONOMICS; RESPONSES; CHOICES; HEALTH; WILL;
D O I
10.1016/j.joep.2021.102471
中图分类号
F [经济];
学科分类号
02 ;
摘要
What motivates runners to bunch at round finishing times? Is it merely the immediate gratification that round time finishing brings, or do athletes set single-stage round time subgoals to help them attain multiple-stage end goals related to their future performance? To examine this question, I construct a new panel data set that covers all finishing times of 7000 major running events of different distances organized in the Netherlands between 1996-2016. For all distances and age groups, I find strong evidence of bunching at round finishing times for both genders, but men engage in round time finishing more frequently than women. No relation is found between round time finishing and running experience. In shorter distance runs, round time finishing is much more prevalent when the reference time is ambitious. This is no proof that runners do not set round time subgoals. It however does suggest that meeting or just missing such a single-race subgoal makes no difference for a runner's future performance in terms of continuing to race, running faster or running longer distances. It also suggests that the excessive amount of round time finishes we observe in all types of races is mainly driven by the immediate gratification (and possibly status) it gives the runner.
引用
收藏
页数:18
相关论文
共 48 条
  • [1] Reference-Dependent Preferences: Evidence from Marathon Runners
    Allen, Eric J.
    Dechow, Patricia M.
    Pope, Devin G.
    Wu, George
    [J]. MANAGEMENT SCIENCE, 2017, 63 (06) : 1657 - 1672
  • [2] [Anonymous], 1999, INTERNAL REVENUE SER
  • [3] Bunching and non-bunching at kink points of the Swedish tax schedule
    Bastani, Spencer
    Selin, Hakan
    [J]. JOURNAL OF PUBLIC ECONOMICS, 2014, 109 : 36 - 49
  • [4] Labor supply of New York City cabdrivers: One day at a time
    Camerer, C
    Babcock, L
    Loewenstein, G
    Thaler, R
    [J]. QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF ECONOMICS, 1997, 112 (02) : 407 - 441
  • [5] ADJUSTMENT COSTS, FIRM RESPONSES, AND MICRO VS. MACRO LABOR SUPPLY ELASTICITIES: EVIDENCE FROM DANISH TAX RECORDS
    Chetty, Raj
    Friedman, John N.
    Olsen, Tore
    Pistaferri, Luigi
    [J]. QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF ECONOMICS, 2011, 126 (02) : 749 - 804
  • [6] Clark R. L., 2020, 28207 NBER
  • [7] New York City Cab Drivers' Labor Supply Revisited: Reference-Dependent Preferences with Rational-Expectations Targets for Hours and Income
    Crawford, Vincent P.
    Meng, Juanjuan
    [J]. AMERICAN ECONOMIC REVIEW, 2011, 101 (05) : 1912 - 1932
  • [8] Taking Stock of Self-Control: A Meta-Analysis of How Trait Self-Control Relates to a Wide Range of Behaviors
    de Ridder, Denise T. D.
    Lensvelt-Mulders, Gerty
    Finkenauer, Catrin
    Stok, F. Marijn
    Baumeister, Roy F.
    [J]. PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW, 2012, 16 (01) : 76 - 99
  • [9] Making complementary choices in consumption episodes: Highlighting versus balancing
    Dhar, R
    Simonson, I
    [J]. JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, 1999, 36 (01) : 29 - 44
  • [10] Will not want: Self-control rather than motivation explains the female advantage in report card grades
    Duckworth, Angela L.
    Shulman, Elizabeth P.
    Mastronarde, Andrew J.
    Patrick, Sarah D.
    Zhang, Jinghui
    Druckman, Jeremy
    [J]. LEARNING AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES, 2015, 39 : 13 - 23