Vernier acuity for stereodisparate objects and ocular prevalence

被引:17
作者
Heinrich, SP [1 ]
Kromeier, M [1 ]
Bach, M [1 ]
Kommerell, G [1 ]
机构
[1] Univ Freiburg, Augenklin, D-79106 Freiburg, Germany
关键词
vernier acuity; position acuity; stereopsis; ocular prevalence; fixation disparity; stereo vision;
D O I
10.1016/j.visres.2004.09.045
中图分类号
Q189 [神经科学];
学科分类号
071006 ;
摘要
Question. How precisely can objects, located in different depth planes, be aligned to the same visual direction? Methods. Twenty normal observers were presented with vertical Vernier lines at various stereodisparities. They had to judge whether the lower, anterior line was located on the right- or left-hand side of the upper, posterior line. Results. Over a stereodisparity range from zero to 62", the threshold for detecting a lateral offset between the Vernier lines remained at the "hyperacuity" level of about 7". With larger stereodisparities, the threshold increased about fourfold, probably due to a mutual, partial suppression of the position signals from the right and left eyes. The reference point from which the observers judged the relative visual directions between stereodisparate objects was not located midway between the eyes; rather, it was often decentred towards the right or the left eye, meaning that the observers had an "ocular prevalence". Their ocular prevalence was, however, not strong enough to have an effect on the Vernier acuity for stereodisparate objects. (Under pathological conditions like strabismic amblyopia, one should expect a 100% prevalence of the good eye, implying that the Vernier acuity reaches the monocular level, irrespective of any depth difference between objects.) Conclusion. Vernier acuity decreases with increasing stereodisparity. Ocular prevalence, occurring frequently among persons with normal eyes, has no effect on Vernier acuity for stereodisparate objects. For a typical everyday viewing condition, the reduced Vernier acuity beyond a stereodisparity of 62" means that, from a viewing distance of 40 cm, precision mechanics have to guide their instrument as close as 0.4 mm to a workpiece, until they can utilise their best position acuity. (c) 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
引用
收藏
页码:1321 / 1328
页数:8
相关论文
共 19 条
[1]   BINOCULAR SUMMATION IN VERNIER ACUITY [J].
BANTON, T ;
LEVI, DM .
JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA A-OPTICS IMAGE SCIENCE AND VISION, 1991, 8 (04) :673-680
[2]   Binocular alignment in different depth planes [J].
Erkelens, CJ ;
Muijs, AJM ;
vanEe, R .
VISION RESEARCH, 1996, 36 (14) :2141-2147
[3]   THRESHOLDS FROM PSYCHOMETRIC FUNCTIONS - SUPERIORITY OF BOOTSTRAP TO INCREMENTAL AND PROBIT VARIANCE ESTIMATORS [J].
FOSTER, DH ;
BISCHOF, WF .
PSYCHOLOGICAL BULLETIN, 1991, 109 (01) :152-159
[4]   BINOCULAR SUMMATION IN HUMANS - EVIDENCE FOR A HIERARCHIC MODEL [J].
FRISEN, L ;
LINDBLOM, B .
JOURNAL OF PHYSIOLOGY-LONDON, 1988, 402 :773-782
[5]   Can fixation disparity be detected by the "Measuring and Correction Methods of H.-J. Haase [J].
Gerling, J ;
de Paz, H ;
Schroth, V ;
Bach, M ;
Kommerell, G .
KLINISCHE MONATSBLATTER FUR AUGENHEILKUNDE, 2000, 216 (06) :401-411
[6]  
Haase H. J., 1995, FIXATIONSDISPARATION
[7]  
HOLM S, 1979, SCAND J STAT, V6, P65
[8]   Ocular prevalence versus ocular dominance [J].
Kommerell, G ;
Schmitt, C ;
Kromeier, M ;
Bach, M .
VISION RESEARCH, 2003, 43 (12) :1397-1403
[10]   Adaptive procedures in psychophysical research [J].
Leek, MR .
PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS, 2001, 63 (08) :1279-1292