A cross-cultural investigation of young children's spontaneous invention of tool use behaviours

被引:19
|
作者
Neldner, Karri [1 ]
Reindl, Eva [2 ,3 ]
Tennie, Claudio [4 ]
Grant, Julie [5 ]
Tomaselli, Keyan [5 ]
Nielsen, Mark [1 ,5 ]
机构
[1] Univ Queensland, Early Cognit Dev Ctr, Sch Psychol, Brisbane, Qld, Australia
[2] Univ Birmingham, Sch Psychol, Birmingham B15 2TT, W Midlands, England
[3] Univ St Andrews, Sch Psychol & Neurosci, St Andrews KY16 9JP, Fife, Scotland
[4] Univ Tubingen, Dept Early Prehist & Quaternary Ecol, Tubingen, Germany
[5] Univ Johannesburg, Dept Commun, Auckland Pk, South Africa
来源
ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE | 2020年 / 7卷 / 05期
基金
澳大利亚研究理事会; 欧洲研究理事会;
关键词
tool use; problem solving; physical cognition; cognitive development; cross-cultural psychology; developmental psychology; INFANT MANUAL EXPLORATION; CUMULATIVE CULTURE; EVOLUTION; PLAY; MANIPULATION; PERSPECTIVE; PSYCHOLOGY; INNOVATION; OBJECTS; SKILL;
D O I
10.1098/rsos.192240
中图分类号
O [数理科学和化学]; P [天文学、地球科学]; Q [生物科学]; N [自然科学总论];
学科分类号
07 ; 0710 ; 09 ;
摘要
Through the mechanisms of observation, imitation and teaching, young children readily pick up the tool using behaviours of their culture. However, little is known about the baseline abilities of children's tool use: what they might be capable of inventing on their own in the absence of socially provided information. It has been shown that children can spontaneously invent 11 of 12 candidate tool using behaviours observed within the foraging behaviours of wild non-human apes (Reindl et al. 2016 Proc. R. Soc. B283, 20152402. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2015.2402)). However, no investigations to date have examined how tool use invention in children might vary across cultural contexts. The current study investigated the levels of spontaneous tool use invention in 2- to 5-year-old children from San Bushmen communities in South Africa and children in a large city in Australia on the same 12 candidate problem-solving tasks. Children in both cultural contexts correctly invented all 12 candidate tool using behaviours, suggesting that these behaviours are within the general cognitive and physical capacities of human children and can be produced in the absence of direct social learning mechanisms such as teaching or observation. Children in both cultures were more likely to invent those tool behaviours more frequently observed in great ape populations than those less frequently observed, suggesting there is similarity in the level of difficulty of invention across these behaviours for all great ape species. However, children in the Australian sample invented tool behaviours and succeeded on the tasks more often than did the Bushmen children, highlighting that aspects of a child's social or cultural environment may influence the rates of their tool use invention on such task sets, even when direct social information is absent.
引用
收藏
页数:14
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [31] A Cross-Cultural Investigation of the Association between Betrayal Trauma and Dissociative Features
    Fung, Hong Wang
    Chien, Wai Tong
    Chan, Chitat
    Ross, Colin A.
    JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, 2023, 38 (1-2) : NP1630 - NP1653
  • [32] A cross-cultural investigation of trait emotional intelligence in Hong Kong and the UK
    Goekcen, Elif
    Furnham, Adrian
    Mavroveli, Stella
    Petrides, K. V.
    PERSONALITY AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES, 2014, 65 : 30 - 35
  • [33] A screening tool for psychological difficulties in children aged 6 to 36 months: cross-cultural validation in Kenya, Cambodia and Uganda
    Nackers, Fabienne
    Roederer, Thomas
    Marquer, Caroline
    Ashaba, Scholastic
    Maling, Samuel
    Mwanga-Amumpaire, Juliet
    Muny, Sothara
    Sokeo, Chea
    Shom, Vireak
    Palha, Maria
    Lefebvre, Pauline
    Kirubi, Beatrice W.
    Kamidigo, Grace
    Falissard, Bruno
    Moro, Marie-Rose
    Grais, Rebecca F.
    BMC PEDIATRICS, 2019, 19 (1)
  • [34] Preschool children's language use in nursery and at home: an investigation of young children's bilingual development
    Gelir, Iskender
    JOURNAL OF MULTILINGUAL AND MULTICULTURAL DEVELOPMENT, 2023, 44 (05) : 388 - 397
  • [35] Parents' literacy on mobile advertising aimed at children: a cross-cultural approach
    Robayo-Pinzon, Oscar
    Rojas-Berrio, Sandra
    Nunez-Gomez, Patricia
    Miguelez-Juan, Blanca
    Garcia-Bejar, Ligia
    YOUNG CONSUMERS, 2022, 23 (02): : 255 - 281
  • [36] Parenting Stress and Social Style in Mothers and Fathers of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Cross-Cultural Investigation in Italy and Japan
    Giannotti, Michele
    Bonatti, Sophia Marlene
    Tanaka, Sanae
    Kojima, Haruyuki
    de Falco, Simona
    BRAIN SCIENCES, 2021, 11 (11)
  • [37] Cross-cultural and psychological adjustment experiences of Bangladeshi students in Japan: a qualitative investigation
    Akon, Md. Saifullah
    Sharmin, Dilruba
    Farsim, Mahbubul Newaz
    Hossain, Sahadat
    CURRENT PSYCHOLOGY, 2025, : 7168 - 7180
  • [38] Cross-Cultural Variation in women's Preferences for men's Body Hair
    Dixson, Barnaby J. W.
    Rantala, Markus J.
    Brooks, Robert C.
    ADAPTIVE HUMAN BEHAVIOR AND PHYSIOLOGY, 2019, 5 (02) : 131 - 147
  • [39] Cross-cultural Continuities and Discontinuities in Shame, Guilt, and Pride: A Study of Children Residing in Japan, Korea and the USA
    Furukawa, Emi
    Tangney, June
    Higashibara, Fumiko
    SELF AND IDENTITY, 2012, 11 (01) : 90 - 113
  • [40] The Community, Autonomy, and Divinity Scale (CADS): A New Tool for the Cross-Cultural Study of Morality
    Guerra, Valeschka M.
    Giner-Sorolla, Roger
    JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2010, 41 (01) : 35 - 50