Alpha-Band Activity Reveals Spontaneous Representations of Spatial Position in Visual Working Memory

被引:107
作者
Foster, Joshua J. [1 ,2 ]
Bsales, Emma M. [1 ,2 ]
Jaffe, Russell J. [1 ,2 ]
Awh, Edward [1 ,2 ]
机构
[1] Univ Chicago, Dept Psychol, Chicago, IL 60637 USA
[2] Univ Chicago, Inst Mind & Biol, Chicago, IL 60637 USA
关键词
SELECTIVE ATTENTION; INFORMATION; LOCATION; ACCESS; MAINTENANCE; PARIETAL; TRACKING; FEATURES; TOOLBOX; BINDING;
D O I
10.1016/j.cub.2017.09.031
中图分类号
Q5 [生物化学]; Q7 [分子生物学];
学科分类号
071010 ; 081704 ;
摘要
An emerging view suggests that spatial position is an integral component of working memory (WM), such that non-spatial features are bound to locations regardless of whether space is relevant [1, 2]. For instance, past work has shown that stimulus position is spontaneously remembered when non-spatial features are stored. Item recognition is enhanced when memoranda appear at the same location where they were encoded [3-5], and accessing non-spatial information elicits shifts of spatial attention to the original position of the stimulus [6, 7]. However, these findings do not establish that a persistent, active representation of stimulus position is maintained in WM because similar effects have also been documented following storage in long-term memory [8, 9]. Here we show that the spatial position of the memorandum is actively coded by persistent neural activity during a non-spatial WM task. We used a spatial encoding model in conjunction with electroencephalogram (EEG) measurements of oscillatory alpha-band (812 Hz) activity to track active representations of spatial position. The position of the stimulus varied trial to trial but was wholly irrelevant to the tasks. We nevertheless observed active neural representations of the original stimulus position that persisted throughout the retention interval. Further experiments established that these spatial representations are dependent on the volitional storage of non-spatial features rather than being a lingering effect of sensory energy or initial encoding demands. These findings provide strong evidence that online spatial representations are spontaneously maintained in WM-regardless of task relevance-during the storage of non-spatial features.
引用
收藏
页码:3216 / +
页数:14
相关论文
共 49 条
[1]  
[Anonymous], 2005, Electric fields of the brain: The neurophysics of eeg
[2]  
[Anonymous], 1993, An introduction to the bootstrap
[3]   Interactions between attention and working memory [J].
Awh, E ;
Vogel, EK ;
Oh, SH .
NEUROSCIENCE, 2006, 139 (01) :201-208
[4]   Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory [J].
Awh, E ;
Jonides, J .
TRENDS IN COGNITIVE SCIENCES, 2001, 5 (03) :119-126
[5]   Rehearsal in spatial working memory [J].
Awh, E ;
Jonides, J ;
Reuter-Lorenz, PA .
JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY-HUMAN PERCEPTION AND PERFORMANCE, 1998, 24 (03) :780-790
[6]   The role of spatial selective attention in working memory for locations: Evidence from event-related potentials [J].
Awh, E ;
Anllo-Vento, L ;
Hillyard, SA .
JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE, 2000, 12 (05) :840-847
[7]   The precision of visual working memory is set by allocation of a shared resource [J].
Bays, Paul M. ;
Catalao, Raquel F. G. ;
Husain, Masud .
JOURNAL OF VISION, 2009, 9 (10)
[8]   The psychophysics toolbox [J].
Brainard, DH .
SPATIAL VISION, 1997, 10 (04) :433-436
[9]   Visual attention: The past 25 years [J].
Carrasco, Marisa .
VISION RESEARCH, 2011, 51 (13) :1484-1525
[10]   The Eyelink Toolbox: Eye tracking with MATLAB and the psychophysics toolbox [J].
Cornelissen, FW ;
Peters, EM ;
Palmer, J .
BEHAVIOR RESEARCH METHODS INSTRUMENTS & COMPUTERS, 2002, 34 (04) :613-617