Voornaam is not (really) a homophone: Lexical prosody and lexical access in Dutch

被引:109
作者
Cutler, A [1 ]
Van Donselaar, W [1 ]
机构
[1] Max Planck Inst Psycholinguist, NL-6500 AH Nijmegen, Netherlands
关键词
Dutch; lexical access; stress; suprasegmentals; word recognition;
D O I
10.1177/00238309010440020301
中图分类号
R36 [病理学]; R76 [耳鼻咽喉科学];
学科分类号
100104 ; 100213 ;
摘要
Four experiments examined Dutch listeners' use of suprasegmental information in spoken-word recognition. Isolated syllables excised from minimal stress pairs such as VOORnaam/voorNAAM could be reliably assigned to their source words. In lexical decision, no priming was observed from one member of minimal stress pairs to the other, suggesting that the pairs' segmental ambiguity was removed by suprasegmental information. Words embedded in nonsense strings were harder to detect if the nonsense string itself formed the beginning of a competing word, but a suprasegmental mismatch to the competing word significantly reduced this inhibition. The same nonsense strings facilitated recognition of the longer words of which they constituted the beginning, but again the facilitation was significantly reduced by suprasegmental mismatch. Together these results indicate that Dutch listeners effectively exploit suprasegmental cues in recognizing spoken words. Nonetheless, suprasegmental mismatch appears to be somewhat less effective in constraining activation than segmental mismatch.
引用
收藏
页码:171 / 195
页数:25
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [31] Syllabic Effects in Italian Lexical Access
    Tagliapietra, Lara
    Fanari, R.
    Collina, S.
    Tabossi, P.
    [J]. JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLINGUISTIC RESEARCH, 2009, 38 (06) : 511 - 526
  • [32] Interactions between lexical access and articulation
    Fink, Angela
    Oppenheim, Gary M.
    Goldrick, Matthew
    [J]. LANGUAGE COGNITION AND NEUROSCIENCE, 2018, 33 (01) : 12 - 24
  • [33] Not all lexical access tasks are created equal: Lexical development between three and five
    Isacoff, Nora M.
    Stromswold, Karin
    [J]. FIRST LANGUAGE, 2014, 34 (01) : 43 - 57
  • [34] The role of lexical-semantic neighborhood in object naming: implications for models of lexical access
    Bormann, Tobias
    [J]. FRONTIERS IN PSYCHOLOGY, 2011, 2
  • [35] Is speech recognition automatic? Lexical competition, but not initial lexical access, requires cognitive resources
    Zhang, Xujin
    Samuel, Arthur G.
    [J]. MUTATION RESEARCH-REVIEWS IN MUTATION RESEARCH, 2018, 775 : 32 - 50
  • [36] Lexical access and competition in bilingual children: The role of proficiency and the lexical similarity of the two languages
    Persici, Valentina
    Vihman, Marilyn
    Burro, Roberto
    Majorano, Marinella
    [J]. JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL CHILD PSYCHOLOGY, 2019, 179 : 103 - 125
  • [37] Combining Acoustic, Lexical, and Syntactic Evidence for Automatic Unsupervised Prosody Labeling
    Ananthakrishnan, Sankaranarayanan
    Narayanan, Shrikanth
    [J]. INTERSPEECH 2006 AND 9TH INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE ON SPOKEN LANGUAGE PROCESSING, VOLS 1-5, 2006, : 297 - 300
  • [38] Metrical Expectations From Preceding Prosody Influence Perception of Lexical Stress
    Brown, Meredith
    Salverda, Anne Pier
    Dilley, Laura C.
    Tanenhaus, Michael K.
    [J]. JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY-HUMAN PERCEPTION AND PERFORMANCE, 2015, 41 (02) : 306 - 323
  • [39] Heeding the voice of experience: The role of talker variation in lexical access
    Creel, Sarah C.
    Aslin, Richard N.
    TanenhauS, Michael K.
    [J]. COGNITION, 2008, 106 (02) : 633 - 664
  • [40] Modeling lexical stress in continuous speech recognition for Dutch
    van den Heuvel, H
    van Kuijk, D
    Boves, L
    [J]. SPEECH COMMUNICATION, 2003, 40 (03) : 335 - 350