Extending Use of the NRT to Preschool-Age Children With and Without Specific Language Impairment

被引:40
作者
Deevy, Patricia [1 ]
Weil, Lisa Wisman [1 ]
Leonard, Laurence B. [1 ]
Goffman, Lisa [1 ]
机构
[1] Purdue Univ, W Lafayette, IN 47907 USA
关键词
nonword repetition; specific language impairment; phonology; diagnostic accuracy; NONWORD REPETITION PERFORMANCE; PHONOLOGICAL MEMORY; PHONOTACTIC PROBABILITY; DIAGNOSTIC-ACCURACY; YOUNG-CHILDREN; PREVALENCE; SPEECH; VOCABULARY; DISORDERS; VALIDITY;
D O I
10.1044/0161-1461(2009/08-0096)
中图分类号
R36 [病理学]; R76 [耳鼻咽喉科学];
学科分类号
100104 ; 100213 ;
摘要
Purpose: The purpose of this study was to assess the diagnostic accuracy of the Nonword Repetition Test (NRT; Dollaghan & Campbell, 1998) using a sample of 4- and 5-year-olds with and without specific language impairment (SLI) and to evaluate its feasibility for use in universal screening. Method: The NRT was administered to 29 children with SLI and 47 age-matched children with typical development. Diagnostic accuracy was computed using alternative scoring methods, which treated out-of-inventory phonemes either as errors or as unscorable. To estimate accuracy in a universal screening context, the probability of identifying a child at risk for language impairment was computed using the prevalence of SLI (7%) as the base rate. Results: Diagnostic accuracy was acceptable using both scoring methods. The resulting likelihood ratios (LR+ = 22.66, 19.43; LR- = .05, .05) were similar to those reported for older children. The probability of accurate detection of children with SLI in the general population increased from 7% to 61%. However, this value suggests that many false positives could be expected. Conclusion: The NRT yielded results similar to those reported for older children. However, despite its strengths, the NRT is not sufficient for screening the general population of 4- and 5-year-olds.
引用
收藏
页码:277 / 288
页数:12
相关论文
共 47 条
[1]  
[Anonymous], EVIDENCE BASED MED T
[2]   Nonword repetition: A comparison of tests [J].
Archibald, Lisa M. D. ;
Gathercole, Susan E. .
JOURNAL OF SPEECH LANGUAGE AND HEARING RESEARCH, 2006, 49 (05) :970-983
[3]  
BANKSON N, 1990, BERNTHAL BANKSON TES
[4]   PREVALENCE OF SPEECH AND LANGUAGE DISORDERS IN 5-YEAR-OLD KINDERGARTEN-CHILDREN IN THE OTTAWA-CARLETON REGION [J].
BEITCHMAN, JH ;
NAIR, R ;
CLEGG, M ;
PATEL, PG .
JOURNAL OF SPEECH AND HEARING DISORDERS, 1986, 51 (02) :98-110
[5]  
Bishop DVM, 2006, GENES BRAIN BEHAV, V5, P158, DOI 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00148.x
[6]   Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: Evidence from a twin study [J].
Bishop, DVM ;
North, T ;
Donlan, C .
JOURNAL OF CHILD PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY AND ALLIED DISCIPLINES, 1996, 37 (04) :391-403
[7]  
Burgemeister B.B., 1972, COLUMBIA MENTAL MATU
[8]   Reducing bias in language assessment: Processing-dependent measures [J].
Campbell, T ;
Dollaghan, C ;
Needleman, H ;
Janosky, J .
JOURNAL OF SPEECH LANGUAGE AND HEARING RESEARCH, 1997, 40 (03) :519-525
[9]   The preschool repetition test: An evaluation of performance in typically developing and clinically referred children [J].
Chiat, Shula ;
Roy, Penny .
JOURNAL OF SPEECH LANGUAGE AND HEARING RESEARCH, 2007, 50 (02) :429-443
[10]  
Cohen J., 1988, Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, VSecond