Decoding Successive Computational Stages of Saliency Processing

被引:68
作者
Bogler, Carsten [1 ,2 ,3 ]
Bode, Stefan [2 ,3 ]
Haynes, John-Dylan [1 ,2 ,3 ]
机构
[1] Charite, Bernstein Ctr Computat Neurosci Berlin, D-10115 Berlin, Germany
[2] Max Planck Inst Human Cognit & Brain Sci, D-04103 Leipzig, Germany
[3] Otto Von Guericke Univ, Dept Neurol, D-39120 Magdeburg, Germany
关键词
LATERAL INTRAPARIETAL AREA; VISUAL SALIENCE; PARIETAL CORTEX; CORTICAL AREA; POP-OUT; ATTENTION; DRIVEN; MAP; RESPONSES; SHIFTS;
D O I
10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.039
中图分类号
Q5 [生物化学]; Q7 [分子生物学];
学科分类号
071010 ; 081704 ;
摘要
An important requirement for vision is to identify interesting and relevant regions of the environment for further processing. Some models assume that salient locations from a visual scene are encoded in a dedicated spatial saliency map [1, 2]. Then, a winner-take-all (WTA) mechanism [1, 2] is often believed to threshold the graded saliency representation and identify the most salient position in the visual field. Here we aimed to assess whether neural representations of graded saliency and the subsequent WTA mechanism can be dissociated. We presented images of natural scenes while subjects were in a scanner performing a demanding fixation task, and thus their attention was directed away. Signals in early visual cortex and posterior intraparietal sulcus (IPS) correlated with graded saliency as defined by a computational saliency model. Multivariate pattern classification [3, 4] revealed that the most salient position in the visual field was encoded in anterior IPS and frontal eye fields (FEF), thus reflecting a potential WTA stage. Our results thus confirm that graded saliency and WTA-thresholded saliency are encoded in distinct neural structures. This could provide the neural representation required for rapid and automatic orientation toward salient events in natural environments.
引用
收藏
页码:1667 / 1671
页数:5
相关论文
共 43 条
[11]   Getting real - sensory processing of natural stimuli [J].
Einhaeuser, Wolfgang ;
Koenig, Peter .
CURRENT OPINION IN NEUROBIOLOGY, 2010, 20 (03) :389-395
[12]   Does luminance-contrast contribute to a saliency map for overt visual attention? [J].
Einhäuser, W ;
König, P .
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF NEUROSCIENCE, 2003, 17 (05) :1089-1097
[13]   Interesting objects are visually salient [J].
Elazary, Lior ;
Itti, Laurent .
JOURNAL OF VISION, 2008, 8 (03)
[14]   Salience, relevance, and firing: a priority map for target selection [J].
Fecteau, Jillian H. ;
Munoz, Douglas. P. .
TRENDS IN COGNITIVE SCIENCES, 2006, 10 (08) :382-390
[15]   On the plausibility of the discriminant center-surround hypothesis for visual saliency [J].
Gao, Dashan ;
Mahadevan, Vijay ;
Vasconcelos, Nuno .
JOURNAL OF VISION, 2008, 8 (07)
[16]   Anterior Intraparietal Sulcus is Sensitive to Bottom-Up Attention Driven by Stimulus Salience [J].
Geng, Joy J. ;
Mangun, George R. .
JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE, 2009, 21 (08) :1584-1601
[17]   Saccades, salience and attention: the role of the lateral intraparietal area in visual behavior [J].
Goldberg, Michael E. ;
Bisley, James W. ;
Powell, Keith D. ;
Gottlieb, Jacqueline .
VISUAL PERCEPTION, PT 2: FUNDAMENTALS OF AWARENESS: MULTI-SENSORY INTEGRATION AND HIGH-ORDER PERCEPTION, 2006, 155 :157-175
[18]   From thought to action: The parietal cortex as a bridge between perception, action, and cognition [J].
Gottlieb, Jacqueline .
NEURON, 2007, 53 (01) :9-16
[19]   The representation of visual salience in monkey parietal cortex [J].
Gottlieb, JP ;
Kusunoki, M ;
Goldberg, ME .
NATURE, 1998, 391 (6666) :481-484
[20]   Decoding mental states from brain activity in humans [J].
Haynes, John-Dylan ;
Rees, Geraint .
NATURE REVIEWS NEUROSCIENCE, 2006, 7 (07) :523-534