Are scalar implicatures automatically processed and different for each individual? A mismatch negativity (MMN) study

被引:19
作者
Zhao, Ming [1 ,2 ]
Liu, Tao [3 ]
Chen, Gang [4 ]
Chen, Feiyan [2 ]
机构
[1] Univ Min & Technol, Sch Literature Law & Polit, Beijing, Peoples R China
[2] Zhejiang Univ, Dept Phys, Bio X Lab, Hangzhou, Zhejiang, Peoples R China
[3] Jiangsu Normal Univ, Jiangsu Key Lab Language & Cognit Neurosci, Xuzhou, Jiangsu, Peoples R China
[4] Anhui Univ, Sch Foreign Studies, Hefei, Peoples R China
关键词
Scalar implicatures; MMN (mismatch negativity); Automatic processing; SN (sustained negativity); Individual differences; Pragmatic abilities; AUDITORY FREQUENCY DISCRIMINATION; PRE-ATTENTIVE DETECTION; HUMAN BRAIN; NEUROPHYSIOLOGICAL EVIDENCE; HEMISPHERIC ASYMMETRIES; WILLIAMS-SYNDROME; TIME-COURSE; LANGUAGE; CHILDREN; MEMORY;
D O I
10.1016/j.brainres.2014.11.049
中图分类号
Q189 [神经科学];
学科分类号
071006 ;
摘要
Scalar implicatures are ordinarily activated in human communication when the speaker uses a weak expression (e.g., some) from a set of stronger alternatives (e.g., many, all). It has been debated whether scalar inferences are generated by default. To clarify this issue and examine whether individual pragmatic ability will affect the mechanism of scalar inference processing, we performed experiment with an MMN paradigm to capture the neurophysiological indicators of automatic processing of spoken sentences and divided participants into high and low pragmatic ability groups. Experimental results showed that compared with the condition that an informative sentence ("Some animals have tails") is the deviant stimuli, when an underinformative sentence ("Some tigers have tails") is the deviant stimuli, the high pragmatic ability group induced mismatch negativity (MMN) and sustained negativity, while the low pragmatic ability group had no ERP effects. These results indicated that at least some people can automatically activate the scalar implicatures when encountering scalar trigger words, even in the inattentive status. (C) 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
引用
收藏
页码:137 / 149
页数:13
相关论文
共 87 条
[71]   QUANTIFICATION AND SEMANTIC MEMORY [J].
RIPS, LJ .
COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY, 1975, 7 (03) :307-340
[72]  
Russell B., 2006, J SEMANT, V23, P361, DOI [10.1093/jos/ffl008, DOI 10.1093/JOS/FFL008]
[73]   AUDITORY FREQUENCY DISCRIMINATION AND EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS [J].
SAMS, M ;
PAAVILAINEN, P ;
ALHO, K ;
NAATANEN, R .
ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAPHY AND CLINICAL NEUROPHYSIOLOGY, 1985, 62 (06) :437-448
[74]   Scalar implicatures in complex sentences [J].
Sauerland, U .
LINGUISTICS AND PHILOSOPHY, 2004, 27 (03) :367-391
[75]   Distributed neuronal networks for encoding category-specific semantic information:: the mismatch negativity to action words [J].
Shtyrov, Y ;
Hauk, O ;
Pulvermüller, F .
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF NEUROSCIENCE, 2004, 19 (04) :1083-1092
[76]   Grammar processing outside the focus of attention:: An MEG study [J].
Shtyrov, Y ;
Pulvermüller, F ;
Näätänen, R ;
Ilmoniemi, RJ .
JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE, 2003, 15 (08) :1195-1206
[77]   Neurophysiological evidence of memory traces for words in the human brain [J].
Shtyrov, Y ;
Pulvermüller, F .
NEUROREPORT, 2002, 13 (04) :521-525
[78]   Early MEG activation dynamics in the left temporal and inferior frontal cortex reflect semantic context integration [J].
Shtyrov, Yury ;
Pulvermueller, Friedemann .
JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE, 2007, 19 (10) :1633-1642
[79]   Interactions between Language and Attention Systems: Early Automatic Lexical Processing? [J].
Shtyrov, Yury ;
Kujala, Teija ;
Pulvermueller, Friedemann .
JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE, 2010, 22 (07) :1465-1478
[80]   Language and conversational abilities in Williams syndrome: How good is good? [J].
Stojanovik, V ;
Perkins, M ;
Howard, S .
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS, 2001, 36 (02) :234-239