Temporal Features of the Differentiation between Self-Name and Religious Leader Name among Christians: An ERP Study

被引:2
作者
Xia, Ruixue [1 ,2 ]
Jin, Ruijie [1 ,2 ]
Yong, Lin [1 ,2 ]
Li, Shaodong [1 ,2 ]
Li, Shifeng [1 ,2 ]
Zhou, Aibao [1 ,2 ]
机构
[1] Northwest Normal Univ, Sch Psychol, Lanzhou, Gansu, Peoples R China
[2] Key Lab Behav & Mental Hlth Gansu Prov, Lanzhou, Gansu, Peoples R China
来源
FRONTIERS IN PSYCHOLOGY | 2018年 / 8卷
基金
中国国家自然科学基金;
关键词
self-name; religious leader; self-concept; P300; ERP; SUBJECTS OWN NAME; SIGNIFICANT OTHERS; FACE RECOGNITION; STIMULI; ATTENTION; BRAIN; P300; FMRI; MEDITATION; REPRESENTATION;
D O I
10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02114
中图分类号
B84 [心理学];
学科分类号
04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
Existing neuroimaging studies have shown that religion, as a subjective culture, can influence self-referential processing. However, the time course of this impact remains unclear. The present study examined how Christians process their own names, the name of their religious leader (i.e., Jesus), and a famous person's name (i.e., Yao Ming). Behavioral and EEG data were recorded while the participants performed a name-color judgment task for these three names. The behavioral data showed no significant differences in reaction time or accuracy among the names. However, the ERP data showed that the P200 and P300 amplitudes elicited by the self-name and religious leader name were larger than those elicited by the famous name. Furthermore, the self-name also elicited a larger P300 amplitude than the religious leader name did. These results suggested that both the self-name and the religious leader name were processed preferentially due to their important social value for the self as compared to a generally famous name. Importantly, the dissociation between the self-name and the religious leader name was observed at a high-order cognitive stage, which might be attributed to their different roles in one's self-concept.
引用
收藏
页数:8
相关论文
共 60 条
  • [1] I, me, mine: Automatic attentional capture by self-related stimuli
    Alexopoulos, Theodore
    Muller, Dominique
    Ric, Francois
    Marendaz, Christian
    [J]. EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2012, 42 (06) : 770 - 779
  • [2] BANAJI MR, 1994, ANNU REV PSYCHOL, V45, P297, DOI 10.1146/annurev.psych.45.1.297
  • [3] P300 IN RESPONSE TO THE SUBJECTS OWN NAME
    BERLAD, I
    PRATT, H
    [J]. EVOKED POTENTIALS-ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAPHY AND CLINICAL NEUROPHYSIOLOGY, 1995, 96 (05): : 472 - 474
  • [4] The timing of cognitive control in partially incongruent categorization
    Chen, Antao
    Xu, Peng
    Wang, Quanhong
    Luo, Yuejia
    Yuan, Jiajin
    Yao, Dezhong
    Li, Hong
    [J]. HUMAN BRAIN MAPPING, 2008, 29 (09) : 1028 - 1039
  • [5] Dynamic Cultural Influences on Neural Representations of the Self
    Chiao, Joan Y.
    Harada, Tokiko
    Komeda, Hidetsugu
    Li, Zhang
    Mano, Yoko
    Saito, Daisuke
    Parrish, Todd B.
    Sadato, Norihiro
    Iidaka, Tetsuya
    [J]. JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE, 2010, 22 (01) : 1 - 11
  • [6] Ching Julia., 1984, BUDDHIST, V4, P31, DOI [DOI 10.2307/1389935, 10.2307/1389935]
  • [7] Affective valence and P300 when stimulus arousal level is controlled
    Conroy, Matthew A.
    Polich, John
    [J]. COGNITION & EMOTION, 2007, 21 (04) : 891 - 901
  • [8] Number-word reading as challenging task in dyslexia?: An ERP study
    Csépe, V
    Szücs, D
    Honbolygó, F
    [J]. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY, 2003, 51 (01) : 69 - 83
  • [9] The effects of ongoing distraction on the neural processes underlying signal detection
    Demeter, Elise
    De Alburquerque, Daniela
    Woldorff, Marty G.
    [J]. NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA, 2016, 89 : 335 - 343
  • [10] Novelty P3 elicited by the subject's own name in comatose patients
    Fischer, Catherine
    Dailler, Frederic
    Morlet, Dominique
    [J]. CLINICAL NEUROPHYSIOLOGY, 2008, 119 (10) : 2224 - 2230