Living on the edge: Effects of body size, group density and microhabitat selection on escape behaviour of southern leopard frogs Lithobates sphenocephalus

被引:10
作者
Bateman, Philip W. [1 ]
Fleming, Patricia A. [2 ]
机构
[1] Curtin Univ, Dept Environm & Agr, Bentley, WA 6845, Australia
[2] Murdoch Univ, Sch Vet & Life Sci, Murdoch, WA 6150, Australia
关键词
Alarm call; Flight initiation distance (FID); Distance fled; Microhabitat selection; Rana sphenocephala; FLIGHT INITIATION DISTANCE; WATER; PERFORMANCE; VOCALIZATION; DIRECTION; ONTOGENY;
D O I
10.1093/czoolo/60.6.712
中图分类号
Q95 [动物学];
学科分类号
071002 ;
摘要
Models of optimal escape strategy predict that animals should move away when the costs of fleeing (metabolic and opportunity costs) are outweighed by the costs of remaining. These theoretical models predict that more vulnerable individuals should be more reactive, moving away when an approaching threat is further away. We tested whether escape behaviour (including 'escape calling') of Lithobates sphenocephalus approached by a human was influenced by body size or the initial microhabitat that the individual was found in. Irrespective of their size, frogs in the open tended to remain immobile, enhancing their crypsis. Frogs in cover showed different responses according to their body size, but, contrary to our initial predictions, larger frogs showed greater responsiveness (longer flight initiation distance and distances fled) than small frogs. Small frogs tended to remain closer to water and escaped into water, while larger individuals were more likely to jump to terrestrial cover and call during escape. Density of frogs near the focal animal had no effect on escape behaviour. This study indicates a range of escape responses in this species and points to the importance of divergent escape choices for organisms which live on the edge of different environments.
引用
收藏
页码:712 / 718
页数:7
相关论文
共 34 条
[1]   The response of water voles, Arvicola terrestris, to the odours of predators [J].
Barreto, GR ;
Macdonald, DW .
ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 1999, 57 :1107-1112
[2]   Who are you looking at? Hadeda ibises use direction of gaze, head orientation and approach speed in their risk assessment of a potential predator [J].
Bateman, P. W. ;
Fleming, P. A. .
JOURNAL OF ZOOLOGY, 2011, 285 (04) :316-323
[3]   Failure to launch? The influence of limb autotomy on the escape behavior of a semiaquatic grasshopper Paroxya atlantica (Acrididae) [J].
Bateman, Philip W. ;
Fleming, Patricia A. .
BEHAVIORAL ECOLOGY, 2011, 22 (04) :763-768
[4]   The influence of distance to burrow on flight initiation distance in the woodchuck, Marmota monax [J].
Bonenfant, M ;
Kramer, DL .
BEHAVIORAL ECOLOGY, 1996, 7 (03) :299-303
[5]   The behavioral response of basking Northern water (Nerodia sipedon) and Eastern garter (Thamnophis sirtalis) snakes to pedestrians in a New Jersey park [J].
Joanna Burger .
Urban Ecosystems, 2001, 5 (2) :119-129
[6]   When to Run and When to Hide: The Influence of Concealment, Visibility, and Proximity to Refugia on Perceptions of Risk [J].
Camp, Meghan J. ;
Rachlow, Janet L. ;
Woods, Bonnie A. ;
Johnson, Timothy R. ;
Shipley, Lisa A. .
ETHOLOGY, 2012, 118 (10) :1010-1017
[7]   VOCAL REPERTOIRE OF BULLFROG (RANA CATESBEIANA) [J].
CAPRANICA, RR .
BEHAVIOUR, 1968, 31 :302-+
[8]   Escape behaviors and flight initiation distance in the common water snake Nerodia sipedon [J].
Cooper, William E., Jr. ;
Attum, Omar ;
Kingsbury, Bruce .
JOURNAL OF HERPETOLOGY, 2008, 42 (03) :493-500
[9]   Effective crypsis and its maintenance by immobility in Craugastor frogs [J].
Cooper, William E., Jr. ;
Caldwell, Janalee P. ;
Vitt, Laurie J. .
COPEIA, 2008, (03) :527-532
[10]   Optimal flight initiation distance [J].
Cooper, William E., Jr. ;
Frederick, William G. .
JOURNAL OF THEORETICAL BIOLOGY, 2007, 244 (01) :59-67