Are tools truly incorporated as an extension of the body representation?: Assessing the evidence for tool embodiment

被引:8
作者
Bell, Joshua D. [1 ]
Macuga, Kristen L. [1 ]
机构
[1] Oregon State Univ, Sch Psychol Sci, 2950 SW Jefferson Way, Corvallis, OR 97330 USA
关键词
Body schema; Tool-use; Tool embodiment; Motor learning; Internal tool models; LIMB POSITION DRIFT; PERIPERSONAL SPACE; INTERNAL-MODELS; MOVEMENT SPEED; ADAPTATION; PERCEPTION; SCHEMA; TOUCH; ARM; RECALIBRATION;
D O I
10.3758/s13423-021-02032-6
中图分类号
B841 [心理学研究方法];
学科分类号
040201 ;
摘要
The predominant view on human tool-use suggests that an action-oriented body representation, the body schema, is altered to fit the tool being wielded, a phenomenon termed tool embodiment. While observations of perceptual change after tool-use purport to support this hypothesis, several issues undermine their validity in this context, discussed at length in this critical review. The primary measures used as indicators of tool embodiment each face unique challenges to their construct validity. Further, the perceptual changes taken as indicating extension of the body representation only appear to account for a fraction of the tool's size in any given experiment, and do not demonstrate the covariance with tool length that the embodiment hypothesis would predict. The expression of tool embodiment also appears limited to a narrow range of tool-use tasks, as deviations from a simple reaching paradigm can mollify or eliminate embodiment effects altogether. The shortcomings identified here generate important avenues for future research. Until the source of the kinematic and perceptual effects that have substantiated tool embodiment is disambiguated, the hypothesis that the body representation changes to fit tools during tool-use should not be favored over other possibilities such as the formation of separable internal tool models, which seem to offer a more complete account of human tool-use behaviors. Indeed, studies of motor learning have observed analogous perceptual changes as aftereffects to adaptation despite the absence of handheld tool-use, offering a compelling alternative explanation, though more work is needed to confirm this possibility.
引用
收藏
页码:343 / 368
页数:26
相关论文
共 127 条
[1]   Tool use and the distalization of the end-effector [J].
Arbib, Michael A. ;
Bonaiuto, James B. ;
Jacobs, Stephane ;
Frey, Scott H. .
PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH-PSYCHOLOGISCHE FORSCHUNG, 2009, 73 (04) :441-462
[2]   Online proprioception feeds plasticity of arm representation following tool-use in healthy aging [J].
Bahmad, Salam ;
Miller, Luke E. ;
Minh Tu Pham ;
Moreau, Richard ;
Salemme, Romeo ;
Koun, Eric ;
Farne, Alessandro ;
Roy, Alice C. .
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS, 2020, 10 (01)
[3]   Dissociating effect of upper limb non-use and overuse on space and body representations [J].
Bassolino, Michela ;
Finisguerra, Alessandra ;
Canzoneri, Elisa ;
Serino, Andrea ;
Pozzo, Thierry .
NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA, 2015, 70 :385-392
[4]   Everyday use of the computer mouse extends peripersonal space representation [J].
Bassolino, Michela ;
Serino, Andrea ;
Ubaldi, Silvia ;
Ladavas, Elisabetta .
NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA, 2010, 48 (03) :803-811
[5]  
Beck BB, 1980, Animal tool behavior: The use and manufacture of tools by animals
[6]   When far becomes near: Remapping of space by tool use [J].
Berti, A ;
Frassinetti, F .
JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE, 2000, 12 (03) :415-420
[7]   This racket is not mine: The influence of the tool-use on peripersonal space [J].
Biggio, Monica ;
Bisio, Ambra ;
Avanzino, Laura ;
Ruggeri, Piero ;
Bove, Marco .
NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA, 2017, 103 :54-58
[8]   Abnormalities in the awareness of action [J].
Blakemore, SJ ;
Wolpert, DM ;
Frith, CD .
TRENDS IN COGNITIVE SCIENCES, 2002, 6 (06) :237-242
[9]  
Bongers RM, 2010, LECT NOTES COMPUT SC, V6192, P271, DOI 10.1007/978-3-642-14075-4_39
[10]   Behavioral performance and visual strategies during skill acquisition using a novel tool use motor learning task [J].
Bosch, T. J. ;
Hanna, T. ;
Fercho, K. A. ;
Baugh, L. A. .
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS, 2018, 8