Preparing learners to self-explain video examples: Text or video introduction?

被引:16
作者
Hefter, Markus H. [1 ]
Berthold, Kirsten [1 ]
机构
[1] Bielefeld Univ, Dept Psychol, Univ Str 25, D-33615 Bielefeld, Germany
关键词
Introductory explanations; Self-explanations; Example-based learning; Video; Text; COGNITIVE-LOAD; ARGUMENTATION SKILLS; WORKED EXAMPLES; MENTAL EFFORT; TRAINING INTERVENTION; EXPLANATIONS; EFFICIENCY; STUDENTS; DESIGN; ACQUISITION;
D O I
10.1016/j.chb.2020.106404
中图分类号
B84 [心理学];
学科分类号
04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
The use of video examples in (online) learning scenarios is rapidly growing and can be very effective for learners with little or no prior knowledge. Previous research identified so-called self-explanation prompts as the method of choice to make learners deeply process the examples' principles. From a cognitive load perspective, it seems feasible to prepare learners for such self-explaining via written explanations and an introductory example-leaving the optimal presentation mode of this introductory example in question. We thus aimed to analyze effectiveness and efficiency of the presentation mode (i.e., written vs. video-based) of the introductory example. In our experiment, students (N = 42) received a written explanation supplemented with an introductory example, before they self-explained video-based examples. We found similar learning processes (i.e., cognitive load and self-explanation quality) and a large effect on learning outcomes (i.e., conceptual knowledge about the examples' principles)-irrespective of the introductory example's presentation mode. However, the written introductory example revealed efficiency advantages over the video-based one: studying it required less time, and recalling its principles required less mental effort. Finally, we identified previous experience with video-based learning as a predictor for learning outcomes.
引用
收藏
页数:9
相关论文
共 56 条
[1]   Transitioning from studying examples to solving problems: Effects of self-explanation prompts and fading worked-out steps [J].
Atkinson, RK ;
Renkl, A ;
Merrill, MM .
JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2003, 95 (04) :774-783
[2]   How to Foster Active Processing of Explanations in Instructional Communication [J].
Berthold, Kirsten ;
Renkl, Alexander .
EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW, 2010, 22 (01) :25-40
[3]   Instructional Aids to Support a Conceptual Understanding of Multiple Representations [J].
Berthold, Kirsten ;
Renkl, Alexander .
JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2009, 101 (01) :70-87
[4]   Assessment of cognitive load in multimedia learning using dual-task methodology [J].
Brünken, R ;
Steinbacher, S ;
Plass, JL ;
Leutner, D .
EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2002, 49 (02) :109-119
[5]  
Busch C., 2008, P 8 INT C LEARN SCI
[6]  
Clark RC, 2011, E-Learning and the Science of Instruction: Proven Guidelines for Consumers and Designers of Multimedia Learning, 3rd Edition, P309
[7]  
Cohen J., 1988, STAT POWER ANAL BEHA, DOI 10.4324/9780203771587
[8]   Improved Effectiveness of Cueing by Self-Explanations when Learning from a Complex Animation [J].
De Koning, Bjorn B. ;
Tabbers, Huib K. ;
Rikers, Remy M. J. P. ;
Paas, Fred .
APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY, 2011, 25 (02) :183-194
[9]   Learning by generating vs. receiving instructional explanations: Two approaches to enhance attention cueing in animations [J].
de Koning, Bjorn B. ;
Tabbers, Huib K. ;
Rikers, Remy M. J. P. ;
Paas, Fred .
COMPUTERS & EDUCATION, 2010, 55 (02) :681-691
[10]   G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences [J].
Faul, Franz ;
Erdfelder, Edgar ;
Lang, Albert-Georg ;
Buchner, Axel .
BEHAVIOR RESEARCH METHODS, 2007, 39 (02) :175-191