When does a system become phonological? Handshape production in gesturers, signers, and homesigners

被引:77
作者
Brentari, Diane [1 ]
Coppola, Marie [2 ,6 ]
Mazzoni, Laura [3 ]
Goldin-Meadow, Susan [4 ,5 ]
机构
[1] Univ Chicago, Dept Linguist, Chicago, IL 60637 USA
[2] Univ Connecticut, Dept Psychol, Storrs, CT USA
[3] Univ Pisa, Dept Linguist, Pisa, Italy
[4] Univ Chicago, Dept Psychol, Chicago, IL 60637 USA
[5] Univ Chicago, Dept Comparat Human Dev, Chicago, IL 60637 USA
[6] Univ Connecticut, Dept Linguist, Storrs, CT USA
基金
美国国家科学基金会;
关键词
Sign language; Phonology; Morphology; Homesign; Gesture; Language evolution; Historical change; Handshape; Classifier predicates; LANGUAGE; EMERGENCE; CHILDREN; GRAMMAR; VERBS;
D O I
10.1007/s11049-011-9145-1
中图分类号
H0 [语言学];
学科分类号
030303 ; 0501 ; 050102 ;
摘要
Sign languages display remarkable crosslinguistic consistencies in the use of handshapes. In particular, handshapes used in classifier predicates display a consistent pattern in finger complexity: classifier handshapes representing objects display more finger complexity than those representing how objects are handled. Here we explore the conditions under which this morphophonological phenomenon arises. In Study 1, we ask whether hearing individuals in Italy and the United States, asked to communicate using only their hands, show the same pattern of finger complexity found in the classifier handshapes of two sign languages: Italian Sign Language (LIS) and American Sign Language (ASL). We find that they do not: gesturers display more finger complexity in handling handshapes than in object handshapes. The morphophonological pattern found in conventional sign languages is therefore not a codified version of the pattern invented by hearing individuals on the spot. In Study 2, we ask whether continued use of gesture as a primary communication system results in a pattern that is more similar to the morphophonological pattern found in conventional sign languages or to the pattern found in gesturers. Homesigners have not acquired a signed or spoken language and instead use a self-generated gesture system to communicate with their hearing family members and friends. We find that homesigners pattern more like signers than like gesturers: their finger complexity in object handshapes is higher than that of gesturers (indeed as high as signers); and their finger complexity in handling handshapes is lower than that of gesturers (but not quite as low as signers). Generally, our findings indicate two markers of the phonologization of handshape in sign languages: increasing finger complexity in object handshapes, and decreasing finger complexity in handling handshapes. These first indicators of phonology appear to be present in individuals developing a gesture system without benefit of a linguistic community. Finally, we propose that iconicity, morphology, and phonology each play an important role in the system of sign language classifiers to create the earliest markers of phonology at the morphophonological interface.
引用
收藏
页码:1 / 31
页数:31
相关论文
共 84 条
[71]   Intergenerational influence and ontogenetic development in the emergence of spatial grammar in Nicaraguan Sign Language [J].
Senghas, A .
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT, 2003, 18 (04) :511-531
[72]   Children creating language: How Nicaraguan sign language acquired a spatial grammar [J].
Senghas, A ;
Coppola, M .
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE, 2001, 12 (04) :323-328
[73]  
Senghas Ann, 1995, PhD dissertation
[74]   ONCE IS NOT ENOUGH - STANDARDS OF WELL-FORMEDNESS IN MANUAL COMMUNICATION CREATED OVER 3 DIFFERENT TIMESPANS [J].
SINGLETON, JL ;
MORFORD, JP ;
GOLDINMEADOW, S .
LANGUAGE, 1993, 69 (04) :683-715
[75]  
STERIADE DONCA., 1995, The_handbook_of phonological_theory, P114, DOI DOI 10.1111/B.9780631201267.1996.00006.X
[76]  
Supalla T., 1982, STRUCTURE ACQUISITIO
[77]  
Supalla Ted, 2008, SIGN LANGUAGES SPINN, P574
[78]  
van der Hulst H., 1995, U TRONDHEIM WORKING, V23, P1
[79]  
Van der Hulst H., 1993, Phonology, V10, P209
[80]  
van der Hulst H., 2006, LAB PHONOL, V8, P265