Measuring mental workload with the NASA-TLX needs to examine each dimension rather than relying on the global score: an example with driving

被引:85
作者
Galy, Edith [1 ]
Paxion, Julie [2 ]
Berthelon, Catherine [3 ]
机构
[1] Univ Cote dAzur, LAPCOS, Nice, France
[2] French Armed Forces Biomed Res Inst, ACSO, Bretigny Sur Orge, France
[3] LMA, TS2, IFSTTAR, Salon De Provence, France
关键词
Mental workload; load dimensions; driving; experience; alertness; COGNITIVE LOAD THEORY; INSTRUCTIONAL-DESIGN; PERFORMANCE; SITUATION; TASK; ACTIVATION; COMPLEXITY; DRIVERS; AROUSAL; SYSTEMS;
D O I
10.1080/00140139.2017.1369583
中图分类号
T [工业技术];
学科分类号
08 ;
摘要
The distinction between several components of mental workload is often made in the ergonomics literature. However, measurements used are often established from a global score, notably with several questionnaires that originally reflect several dimensions. The present study tested the effect of driving situation complexity, experience and subjective levels of tension and alertness on each dimension of the NASA-TLX questionnaire of workload, in order to highlight the potential influence of intrinsic, extraneous and germane load factors. The results showed that, in complex situation, mental, temporal and physical demand (load dimensions) increased, and that novice drivers presented high physical demand when subjective tension was low on performance. Moreover, increase of mental and physical demand increased effort. It thus, appears essential to distinguish the different components of mental workload used in the NASA-TLX questionnaire.Practitioner Summary: Currently, global score of NASA-TLX questionnaire is used to measure mental workload. Here, we considered independently each dimension of NASA-TLX, and results showed that mental load factors (driving situation complexity, experience, subjective tension and alertness) had a different effect on dimensions, questioning global score use to evaluate workload.
引用
收藏
页码:517 / 527
页数:11
相关论文
共 55 条
[1]  
AMALBERTI R, 1996, CONDUITE SYSTEMES RI
[2]   Analyzing the structure of expert knowledge [J].
Bradley, JH ;
Paul, R ;
Seeman, E .
INFORMATION & MANAGEMENT, 2006, 43 (01) :77-91
[3]   Correlation between driving errors and vigilance level: influence of the driver's age [J].
Campagne, A ;
Pebayle, T ;
Muzet, A .
PHYSIOLOGY & BEHAVIOR, 2004, 80 (04) :515-524
[4]   Cognitive styles as an explanation of experts' individual differences: A case study in computer-assisted troubleshooting diagnosis [J].
Cegarra, J ;
Hoc, JM .
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF HUMAN-COMPUTER STUDIES, 2006, 64 (02) :123-136
[5]   The use of Tholos software for combining measures of mental workload: Toward theoretical and methodological improvements [J].
Cegarra, Julien ;
Chevalier, Aline .
BEHAVIOR RESEARCH METHODS, 2008, 40 (04) :988-1000
[6]  
Chanquoy Lucile, 2007, the cognitive load: theories and applications
[7]   Autonomic nervous system and subjective ratings of strain in air-traffic control [J].
Collet, C. ;
Averty, P. ;
Dittmar, A. .
APPLIED ERGONOMICS, 2009, 40 (01) :23-32
[8]   Stroop color-word test, arousal, electrodermal activity and performance in a critical driving situation [J].
Collet, C ;
Petit, C ;
Priez, A ;
Dittmar, A .
BIOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 69 (02) :195-203
[9]   The Evaluation of Traditional and Early Driver Training With Simulated Accident Scenarios [J].
Damm, Loic ;
Nachtergaele, Claudine ;
Meskali, Mohamed ;
Berthelon, Catherine .
HUMAN FACTORS, 2011, 53 (04) :323-337
[10]   The development of a method to measure speed adaptation to traffic complexity: Identifying novice, unsafe, and overconfident drivers [J].
de Craen, Saskia ;
Twisk, Divera A. M. ;
Hagenzieker, Marjan P. ;
Elffers, Henk ;
Brookhuis, Karel A. .
ACCIDENT ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION, 2008, 40 (04) :1524-1530