Representation and disconnection in imaginal neglect

被引:37
|
作者
Rode, G. [1 ,2 ]
Cotton, F. [3 ]
Revol, P. [1 ,2 ]
Jacquin-Courtois, S. [1 ,2 ]
Rossetti, Y. [1 ,2 ]
Bartolomeo, P. [4 ,5 ,6 ]
机构
[1] Univ Lyon 1, INSERM, UMRS 534, Bron, France
[2] Hop Henry Gabrielle, Hosp Civils Lyon, Serv Med Phys & Readaptat Neurol, Lyon, France
[3] Univ Lyon 1, Serv Radiol, Ctr Hosp Lyon Sud, Hosp Civils Lyon, F-69365 Lyon, France
[4] UPMC, INSERM, UMRS 975, Paris, France
[5] Hop La Pitie Salpetriere, AP HP, Paris, France
[6] Catholic Univ, Dept Psychol, Milan, Italy
关键词
Visual neglect; Representational neglect; Spatial imagery; Top-down mechanisms; SPATIAL AWARENESS; UNILATERAL NEGLECT; VISUAL NEGLECT; PRISM ADAPTATION; BRAIN NETWORKS; ATTENTION; PARIETAL; HEMINEGLECT; EVOCATION; REMISSION;
D O I
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.05.032
中图分类号
B84 [心理学]; C [社会科学总论]; Q98 [人类学];
学科分类号
03 ; 0303 ; 030303 ; 04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
Patients with neglect failure to detect, orient, or respond to stimuli from a spatially confined region, usually on their left side. Often, the presence of perceptual input increases left omissions, while sensory deprivation decreases them, possibly by removing attention-catching right-sided stimuli (Bartolomeo, 2007). However, such an influence of visual deprivation on representational neglect was not observed in patients while they were imagining a map of France (Rode et al., 2007). Therefore, these patients with imaginal neglect either failed to generate the left side of mental images (Bisiach & Luzzatti, 1978), or suffered from a co-occurrence of deficits in automatic (bottom-up) and voluntary (top-down) orienting of attention. However, in Rode et al.'s experiment visual input was not directly relevant to the task; moreover, distraction from visual input might primarily manifest itself when representation guides somatornotor actions, beyond those involved in the generation and mental exploration of an internal map (Thomas, 1999). To explore these possibilities, we asked a patient with right hemisphere damage, R.D., to explore visual and imagined versions of a map of France in three conditions: (1) 'imagine the map in your mind' (imaginal); (2) 'describe a real map' (visual); and (3) 'list the names of French towns' (propositional). For the imaginal and visual conditions, verbal and manual pointing responses were collected; the task was also given before and after mental rotation of the map by 180. RD. mentioned more towns on the right side of the map in the imaginal and visual conditions, but showed no representational deficit in the propositional condition. The rightward inner exploration bias in the imaginal and visual conditions was similar in magnitude and was not influenced by mental rotation or response type (verbal responses or manual pointing to locations on a map), thus suggesting that the representational deficit was robust and independent of perceptual input in RD. Structural and diffusion MRI demonstrated damage to several white matter tracts in the right hemisphere and to the splenium of corpus callosum. A second right-brain damaged patient (P.P.), who showed signs of visual but not imaginal neglect, had damage to the same intra-hemispheric tracts, but the callosal connections were spared. Imaginal neglect in R.D. may result from fronto-parietal dysfunction impairing orientation towards left-sided items and posterior callosal disconnection preventing the symmetrical processing of spatial information from long-term memory. (C) 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
引用
收藏
页码:2903 / 2911
页数:9
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [31] The spatial representation of numbers: evidence from neglect and pseudoneglect
    Umilta, Carlo
    Priftis, Konstantinos
    Zorzi, Marco
    EXPERIMENTAL BRAIN RESEARCH, 2009, 192 (03) : 561 - 569
  • [32] Repetitive exploration towards locations that no longer carry a target in patients with neglect
    Nys, Gudrun M. S.
    Stuart, Marije
    Dijkerman, H. Chris
    JOURNAL OF NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2010, 4 : 33 - 45
  • [33] Prism adaptation for spatial neglect after stroke: translational practice gaps
    Barrett, A. M.
    Goedert, Kelly M.
    Basso, Julia C.
    NATURE REVIEWS NEUROLOGY, 2012, 8 (10) : 567 - 577
  • [34] A simple measure of neglect severity
    Rorden, Christopher
    Karnath, Hans-Otto
    NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA, 2010, 48 (09) : 2758 - 2763
  • [35] Hemispatial neglect: approaches to rehabilitation
    Singh-Curry, Victoria
    Husain, Masud
    CLINICAL REHABILITATION, 2010, 24 (08) : 675 - 684
  • [36] Aligning formal and functional assessments of Visuospatial Neglect: A mixed-methods study
    Moore, Margaret Jane
    Driscoll, Rebecca
    Colwell, Michael
    Hewitt, Olivia
    Demeyere, Nele
    NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL REHABILITATION, 2022, 32 (10) : 2560 - 2579
  • [37] Line bisection under an attentional gradient induced by simulated neglect in healthy subjects
    Grewal, Parampal
    Viswanathan, Jayalakshmi
    Barton, Jason J. S.
    Lanyon, Linda J.
    NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA, 2012, 50 (06) : 1190 - 1201
  • [38] Implicit representation and explicit detection of features in patients with hemispatial neglect
    Van Vleet, Thomas M.
    Robertson, Lynn C.
    BRAIN, 2009, 132 : 1889 - 1897
  • [39] Biased figure-ground assignment affects conscious object recognition in spatial neglect
    Eramudugolla, Ranmalee
    Driver, Jon
    Mattingley, Jason B.
    COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE, 2010, 1 (03) : 155 - 164
  • [40] Deficits of Contralesional Awareness: A Case Study on What Paper-and-Pencil Tests Neglect
    Bonato, Mario
    Priftis, Konstantinos
    Marenzi, Roberto
    Umilta, Carlo
    Zorzi, Marco
    NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2012, 26 (01) : 20 - 36