Representation and disconnection in imaginal neglect

被引:37
|
作者
Rode, G. [1 ,2 ]
Cotton, F. [3 ]
Revol, P. [1 ,2 ]
Jacquin-Courtois, S. [1 ,2 ]
Rossetti, Y. [1 ,2 ]
Bartolomeo, P. [4 ,5 ,6 ]
机构
[1] Univ Lyon 1, INSERM, UMRS 534, Bron, France
[2] Hop Henry Gabrielle, Hosp Civils Lyon, Serv Med Phys & Readaptat Neurol, Lyon, France
[3] Univ Lyon 1, Serv Radiol, Ctr Hosp Lyon Sud, Hosp Civils Lyon, F-69365 Lyon, France
[4] UPMC, INSERM, UMRS 975, Paris, France
[5] Hop La Pitie Salpetriere, AP HP, Paris, France
[6] Catholic Univ, Dept Psychol, Milan, Italy
关键词
Visual neglect; Representational neglect; Spatial imagery; Top-down mechanisms; SPATIAL AWARENESS; UNILATERAL NEGLECT; VISUAL NEGLECT; PRISM ADAPTATION; BRAIN NETWORKS; ATTENTION; PARIETAL; HEMINEGLECT; EVOCATION; REMISSION;
D O I
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.05.032
中图分类号
B84 [心理学]; C [社会科学总论]; Q98 [人类学];
学科分类号
03 ; 0303 ; 030303 ; 04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
Patients with neglect failure to detect, orient, or respond to stimuli from a spatially confined region, usually on their left side. Often, the presence of perceptual input increases left omissions, while sensory deprivation decreases them, possibly by removing attention-catching right-sided stimuli (Bartolomeo, 2007). However, such an influence of visual deprivation on representational neglect was not observed in patients while they were imagining a map of France (Rode et al., 2007). Therefore, these patients with imaginal neglect either failed to generate the left side of mental images (Bisiach & Luzzatti, 1978), or suffered from a co-occurrence of deficits in automatic (bottom-up) and voluntary (top-down) orienting of attention. However, in Rode et al.'s experiment visual input was not directly relevant to the task; moreover, distraction from visual input might primarily manifest itself when representation guides somatornotor actions, beyond those involved in the generation and mental exploration of an internal map (Thomas, 1999). To explore these possibilities, we asked a patient with right hemisphere damage, R.D., to explore visual and imagined versions of a map of France in three conditions: (1) 'imagine the map in your mind' (imaginal); (2) 'describe a real map' (visual); and (3) 'list the names of French towns' (propositional). For the imaginal and visual conditions, verbal and manual pointing responses were collected; the task was also given before and after mental rotation of the map by 180. RD. mentioned more towns on the right side of the map in the imaginal and visual conditions, but showed no representational deficit in the propositional condition. The rightward inner exploration bias in the imaginal and visual conditions was similar in magnitude and was not influenced by mental rotation or response type (verbal responses or manual pointing to locations on a map), thus suggesting that the representational deficit was robust and independent of perceptual input in RD. Structural and diffusion MRI demonstrated damage to several white matter tracts in the right hemisphere and to the splenium of corpus callosum. A second right-brain damaged patient (P.P.), who showed signs of visual but not imaginal neglect, had damage to the same intra-hemispheric tracts, but the callosal connections were spared. Imaginal neglect in R.D. may result from fronto-parietal dysfunction impairing orientation towards left-sided items and posterior callosal disconnection preventing the symmetrical processing of spatial information from long-term memory. (C) 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
引用
收藏
页码:2903 / 2911
页数:9
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [21] Complexity vs. unity in unilateral spatial neglect
    Rode, G.
    Fourtassi, M.
    Pagliari, C.
    Pisella, L.
    Rossetti, Y.
    REVUE NEUROLOGIQUE, 2017, 173 (7-8) : 440 - 450
  • [22] Unilateral neglect: A review of causes, anatomical localization, theories and interventions
    Saevarsson, Styrmir
    Kristjansson, Arni
    Hjaltason, Haukur
    LAEKNABLADID, 2009, 95 (01): : 27 - 33
  • [23] Association of disruption of the right posterior arcuate fasciculus with spatial neglect
    Andreoli, Mia
    Medina-Pizarro, Mauricio
    Mackie, Melissa-Ann
    Tate, Matthew C.
    JOURNAL OF NEUROSURGERY, 2024, 141 (03) : 624 - 633
  • [24] Damage to the medial motor system in stroke patients motor neglect
    Migliaccio, Raffaella
    Bouhali, Florence
    Rastelli, Federica
    Ferrieux, Sophie
    Arbizu, Celine
    Vincent, Stephane
    Pradat-Diehl, Pascale
    Bartolomeo, Paolo
    FRONTIERS IN HUMAN NEUROSCIENCE, 2014, 8
  • [25] Near and far space neglect: Task sensitivity and anatomical substrates
    Aimola, Lina
    Schindler, Igor
    Simone, Anna Maria
    Venneri, Annalena
    NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA, 2012, 50 (06) : 1115 - 1123
  • [26] Coupling Acoustic Feedback to Eye Movements to Reduce Spatial Neglect
    Pusswald, Gisela
    Steinhoff, Nikolaus
    Mueller, Christian
    TOPICS IN STROKE REHABILITATION, 2013, 20 (03) : 262 - 269
  • [27] The effects of the dopamine agonist rotigotine on hemispatial neglect following stroke
    Gorgoraptis, Nikos
    Mah, Yee-Haur
    Machner, Bjoern
    Singh-Curry, Victoria
    Malhotra, Paresh
    Hadji-Michael, Maria
    Cohen, David
    Simister, Robert
    Nair, Ajoy
    Kulinskaya, Elena
    Ward, Nick
    Greenwood, Richard
    Husain, Masud
    BRAIN, 2012, 135 : 2478 - 2491
  • [28] LEFT IMAGINAL NEGLECT IN HEMINATTENTION - EXPERIMENTAL-STUDY WITH THE OCLOCK TEST
    GROSSI, D
    ANGELINI, R
    PECCHINENDA, A
    PIZZAMIGLIO, L
    BEHAVIOURAL NEUROLOGY, 1993, 6 (03) : 155 - 158
  • [29] Neglect: A multisensory deficit?
    Jacobs, Stephane
    Brozzoli, Claudio
    Farne, Alessandro
    NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA, 2012, 50 (06) : 1029 - 1044
  • [30] Body representation alterations in personal but not in extrapersonal neglect patients
    Di Vita, Antonella
    Palermo, Liana
    Piccardi, Laura
    Di Tella, Jessica
    Propato, Filomena
    Guariglia, Cecilia
    APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY-ADULT, 2017, 24 (04) : 308 - 317