The effects of local prevalence and explicit expectations on search termination times

被引:39
作者
Ishibashi, Kazuya [1 ,2 ]
Kita, Shinichi [1 ]
Wolfe, Jeremy M. [3 ,4 ]
机构
[1] Kobe Univ, Grad Sch Humanities, Nada Ku, Kobe, Hyogo 657, Japan
[2] Chinese Univ Hong Kong, Hong Kong, Hong Kong, Peoples R China
[3] Brigham & Womens Hosp, Boston, MA 02115 USA
[4] Harvard Univ, Sch Med, Boston, MA USA
基金
日本学术振兴会; 美国国家卫生研究院;
关键词
Visual search; Prevalence effect; Search termination time; Explicit expectation; VISUAL-SEARCH; RARE;
D O I
10.3758/s13414-011-0225-4
中图分类号
B84 [心理学];
学科分类号
04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
In visual search tasks, the relative proportions of target-present and -absent trials have important effects on behavior. Miss error rates rise as target prevalence decreases (Wolfe, Horowitz, & Kenner, Nature 435, 439-440, 2005). At the same time, search termination times on target-absent trials become shorter (Wolfe & Van Wert, Current Biology 20, 121-124, 2010). These effects must depend on some implicit or explicit knowledge of the current prevalence. What is the nature of that knowledge? In Experiment 1, we conducted visual search tasks at three levels of prevalence (6%, 50%, and 94%) and analyzed performance as a function of "local prevalence," the prevalence over the last n trials. The results replicated the usual effects of overall prevalence but revealed only weak or absent effects of local prevalence. In Experiment 2, the overall prevalence in a block of trials was 20%, 50%, or 80%. However, a 100%-valid cue informed observers of the prevalence on the next trial. These explicit cues had a modest effect on target-absent RTs, but explicit expectation could not explain the full prevalence effect. We conclude that observers predict prevalence on the basis of an assessment of a relatively long prior history. Each trial contributes a small amount to that assessment, and this can be modulated but not overruled by explicit instruction.
引用
收藏
页码:115 / 123
页数:9
相关论文
共 17 条
[1]   Apostatic selection by blue jays produces balanced polymorphism in virtual prey [J].
Bond, AB ;
Kamil, AC .
NATURE, 1998, 395 (6702) :594-596
[2]   The psychophysics toolbox [J].
Brainard, DH .
SPATIAL VISION, 1997, 10 (04) :433-436
[3]  
Egeth Howard., 1977, Psychology of Learning and Motivation, V11, P277, DOI DOI 10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60480-6
[4]   Rare targets are rarely missed in correctable search [J].
Fleck, Mathias S. ;
Mitroff, Stephen R. .
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE, 2007, 18 (11) :943-947
[5]   Dual-target search for high and low prevalence X-ray threat targets [J].
Godwin, Hayward J. ;
Menneer, Tamaryn ;
Cave, Kyle R. ;
Donnelly, Nick .
VISUAL COGNITION, 2010, 18 (10) :1439-1463
[6]   Changes in breast cancer detection and mammography recall rates after the introduction of a computer-aided detection system [J].
Gur, D ;
Sumkin, JH ;
Rockette, HE ;
Ganott, M ;
Hakim, C ;
Hardesty, L ;
Poller, WR ;
Shah, R ;
Wallace, L .
JOURNAL OF THE NATIONAL CANCER INSTITUTE, 2004, 96 (03) :185-190
[7]   PROBABILITY MATCHING AND THE FORMATION OF CONSERVATIVE DECISION RULES IN A NUMERICAL ANALOG OF SIGNAL-DETECTION [J].
HEALY, AF ;
KUBOVY, M .
JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY-HUMAN LEARNING AND MEMORY, 1981, 7 (05) :344-354
[8]   The role of priming in conjunctive visual search [J].
Kristjánsson, A ;
Wang, DL ;
Nakayama, K .
COGNITION, 2002, 85 (01) :37-52
[9]   The prevalence effect is determined by past experience, not future prospects [J].
Lau, Jonas Sin-Heng ;
Huang, Liqiang .
VISION RESEARCH, 2010, 50 (15) :1469-1474
[10]   PRIMING OF POP-OUT .1. ROLE OF FEATURES [J].
MALJKOVIC, V ;
NAKAYAMA, K .
MEMORY & COGNITION, 1994, 22 (06) :657-672