Time course of allocentric decay, egocentric decay, and allocentric-to-egocentric conversion in memory-guided reach

被引:47
作者
Chen, Ying [1 ,2 ,3 ]
Byrne, Patrick [1 ,2 ]
Crawford, J. Douglas [1 ,2 ,3 ,4 ,5 ,6 ]
机构
[1] York Univ, Ctr Vis Res, Toronto, ON M3J 1P3, Canada
[2] Canadian Act & Percept Network CAPnet, Toronto, ON, Canada
[3] York Univ, Sch Kinesiol & Hlth Sci, Toronto, ON M3J 1P3, Canada
[4] York Univ, Neurosci Grad Diploma Program, Toronto, ON M3J 1P3, Canada
[5] York Univ, Dept Psychol, Toronto, ON M3J 1P3, Canada
[6] York Univ, Dept Biol, Toronto, ON M3J 1P3, Canada
关键词
Memory-guided reaching; Egocentric representation; Allocentric representation; Allocentric-to-egocentric conversion; REMEMBERED VISUAL SPACE; ACCURACY TRADE-OFF; CORTICAL CONNECTIONS; INTRAPARIETAL CORTEX; POINTING MOVEMENTS; EYE-MOVEMENTS; PARIETAL; REPRESENTATION; INFORMATION; PERCEPTION;
D O I
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.10.031
中图分类号
B84 [心理学]; C [社会科学总论]; Q98 [人类学];
学科分类号
03 ; 0303 ; 030303 ; 04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
Allocentric cues can be used to encode locations in visuospatial memory, but it is not known how and when these representations are converted into egocentric commands for behaviour. Here, we tested the influence of different memory intervals on reach performance toward targets defined in either egocentric or allocentric coordinates, and then compared this to performance in a task where subjects were implicitly free to choose when to convert from allocentric to egocentric representations. Reach and eye positions were measured using Optotrak and Eyelink Systems, respectively, in fourteen subjects. Our results confirm that egocentric representations degrade over a delay of several seconds, whereas allocentric representations remained relatively stable over the same time scale. Moreover, when subjects were free to choose, they converted allocentric representations into egocentric representations as soon as possible, despite the apparent cost in reach precision in our experimental paradigm. This suggests that humans convert allocentric representations into egocentric commands at the first opportunity, perhaps to optimize motor noise and movement timing in real-world conditions. (C) 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
引用
收藏
页码:49 / 60
页数:12
相关论文
共 77 条
[1]   Intentional maps in posterior parietal cortex [J].
Andersen, RA ;
Buneo, CA .
ANNUAL REVIEW OF NEUROSCIENCE, 2002, 25 :189-220
[2]  
Baddeley A. D., 1986, WORKING MEMORY
[3]   Reach plans in eye-centered coordinates [J].
Batista, AP ;
Buneo, CA ;
Snyder, LH ;
Andersen, RA .
SCIENCE, 1999, 285 (5425) :257-260
[4]   Decay in visuomotor representations during manual aiming [J].
Binsted, G ;
Rolheiser, TM ;
Chua, R .
JOURNAL OF MOTOR BEHAVIOR, 2006, 38 (02) :82-87
[5]   Decoding the Cortical Transformations for Visually Guided Reaching in 3D Space [J].
Blohm, Gunnar ;
Keith, Gerald P. ;
Crawford, J. Douglas .
CEREBRAL CORTEX, 2009, 19 (06) :1372-1393
[6]  
BOCK O, 1986, EXP BRAIN RES, V62, P451
[7]  
BOCK O, 1993, EXP BRAIN RES, V95, P111
[8]   CONTRIBUTION OF RETINAL VERSUS EXTRARETINAL SIGNALS TOWARDS VISUAL LOCALIZATION IN GOAL-DIRECTED MOVEMENTS [J].
BOCK, O .
EXPERIMENTAL BRAIN RESEARCH, 1986, 64 (03) :476-482
[9]  
BOOTSMA RJ, 1994, EXP BRAIN RES, V98, P535
[10]   A dissociation of perception and action in normal human observers: the effect of temporal-delay [J].
Bradshaw, MF ;
Watt, SJ .
NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA, 2002, 40 (11) :1766-1778