Intergenerational effects of mother's early rearing experience on offspring treatment and socioemotional development

被引:11
作者
Bassett, Ashley M. Sproul [1 ,5 ]
Wood, Elizabeth K. [1 ]
Lindell, Stephen G. [2 ,3 ]
Schwandt, Melanie L. [3 ]
Barr, Christina S. [2 ,3 ]
Suomi, Stephen J. [4 ]
Higley, James D. [1 ]
机构
[1] Brigham Young Univ, Dept Psychol, 1042 KMBL, Provo, UT 84602 USA
[2] NIAAA, Sect Comparat Behav Genom, NIH, Rockville, MD 20852 USA
[3] NIAAA, Clin Studies Lab, NIH, Bethesda, MD 20892 USA
[4] Eunice Shriver Kennedy Natl Inst Child Hlth & Hum, Sect Comparat Ethol, NIH, Poolesville, MD USA
[5] Rocky Mt Univ Hlth Profess, Provo, UT 84606 USA
关键词
early experience; intergenerational effects; mother-infant relations; rhesus monkeys; sensitive parenting; MONOAMINE METABOLITE LEVELS; SEROTONIN TRANSPORTER GENE; RHESUS MACAQUES; MATERNAL-BEHAVIOR; ATTACHMENT SECURITY; CHILD MALTREATMENT; INFANT ATTACHMENT; PRIMATE MODELS; GENERATIONS; TRANSMISSION;
D O I
10.1002/dev.21959
中图分类号
Q [生物科学];
学科分类号
07 ; 0710 ; 09 ;
摘要
This longitudinal study spans two generations of rhesus monkeys. First, the study investigates the effects of early rearing experiences on the maternal behavior of first-generation mothers (rates of premature infant rejection) and, second, the study investigates the effects of maternal rejection on the behavior of second-generation infants. Rhesus macaque mother-infant dyads (Macaca mulatta-N = 176) were observed twice weekly, with each session lasting 300 s. First-generation mothers were raised in one of three conditions: as mother-reared controls (MR; [n = 95]), in peer groups (PR; raised without adults but with constant access to three same-aged peers [n = 49]), or with an inanimate surrogate (SPR; raised with an inanimate fleece-covered, surrogate mother and limited daily peer-group interactions [n = 32]). Second-generation infants were all raised by their differentially reared mothers and statistically grouped into one of two groups: those that were rejected by their mothers beginning at a more-typical weaning age (controls), starting in the third month of life (n = 108), and those that were prematurely rejected, with mothers showing rejections before the third month of infant life (n = 68). Overall, PR mothers exhibited the highest rates of premature infant rejection, except for month 1 of infant life, when SPR mothers exhibited the highest rates of rejection. Intriguingly, after month 1, SPR mothers showed high rates of infant cradling and seldom rejected their infants. Independent of their mothers' early rearing environment, prematurely rejected infants displayed more aggression and passive vigilance, and were cradled and groomed less by their mothers, and there was evidence that the overall rates of rejection after the first 2 months of life had a cumulative negative effect on the developing infant.Post hocanalyses of plasma cortisol levels showed that the prematurely rejected infants had higher cortisol concentrations, suggesting a high level of stress in the prematurely rejected infants. These results suggest that maternal presence during infancy has long-term effects on a female's future maternal skills which, in turn, have intergenerational consequences for the socioemotional development of second-generation infants.
引用
收藏
页码:920 / 931
页数:12
相关论文
共 68 条
[1]  
ANDREWS MW, 1991, CHILD DEV, V62, P686, DOI 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01562.x
[2]  
ANDREWS MW, 1993, J COMP PSYCHOL, V107, P84
[3]   Parenting Practices and Problem Behavior Across Three Generations: Monitoring, Harsh Discipline, and Drug Use in the Intergenerational Transmission of Externalizing Behavior [J].
Bailey, Jennifer A. ;
Hill, Karl G. ;
Oesterle, Sabrina ;
Hawkins, J. David .
DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2009, 45 (05) :1214-1226
[4]   Relationships among CSF monoamine metabolite levels, alcohol sensitivity, and alcohol-related aggression in rhesus macaques [J].
Barr, CS ;
Becker, ML ;
Suomi, SJ ;
Higley, JD .
AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOR, 2003, 29 (04) :288-301
[5]   Intergenerational transmission of warm-sensitive-stimulating parenting: A prospective study of mothers and fathers of 3-year-olds [J].
Belsky, J ;
Jaffee, SR ;
Sligo, J ;
Woodward, L ;
Silva, PA .
CHILD DEVELOPMENT, 2005, 76 (02) :384-396
[6]   ETIOLOGY OF CHILD MALTREATMENT - A DEVELOPMENTAL ECOLOGICAL ANALYSIS [J].
BELSKY, J .
PSYCHOLOGICAL BULLETIN, 1993, 114 (03) :413-434
[7]   Early experience and serotonin transporter gene variation interact to influence primate CNS function [J].
Bennett, AJ ;
Lesch, KP ;
Heils, A ;
Long, JC ;
Lorenz, JG ;
Shoaf, SE ;
Champoux, M ;
Suomi, SJ ;
Linnoila, MV ;
Higley, JD .
MOLECULAR PSYCHIATRY, 2002, 7 (01) :118-122
[8]   Nurturing care: promoting early childhood development [J].
Britto, Pia R. ;
Lye, Stephen J. ;
Proulx, Kerrie ;
Yousafzai, Aisha K. ;
Matthews, Stephen G. ;
Vaivada, Tyler ;
Perez-Escamilla, Rafael ;
Rao, Nirmala ;
Ip, Patrick ;
Fernald, Lia C. H. ;
MacMillan, Harriet ;
Hanson, Mark ;
Wachs, Theodore D. ;
Yao, Haogen ;
Yoshikawa, Hirokazu ;
Cerezo, Adrian ;
Leckman, James F. ;
Bhutta, Zulfiqar A. .
LANCET, 2017, 389 (10064) :91-102
[9]   The development of behavioural sex differences in infant rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) [J].
Brown, GR ;
Dixson, AF .
PRIMATES, 2000, 41 (01) :63-77
[10]  
Capitanio J. P., 2018, BEHAV INHIBITION INT, P17, DOI [10.1007/978-3-319-98077-5_2, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-98077-5_2]