Imagining What Could Have Happened: Types and Vividness of Counterfactual Thoughts and the Relationship With Post-traumatic Stress Reactions

被引:17
作者
Blix, Ines [1 ]
Kanten, Alf Borre [2 ,3 ]
Birkeland, Marianne Skogbrott [1 ]
Thoresen, Siri [1 ]
机构
[1] Norwegian Ctr Violence & Traumat Stress Studies, Oslo, Norway
[2] Univ Oslo, Fac Social Sci, Dept Psychol, Oslo, Norway
[3] Bjorknes Coll, Oslo, Norway
关键词
post-traumatic stress; counterfactual thinking; trauma; PTSD; cognition; PSYCHOLOGICAL DISTRESS; COPING STRATEGIES; THINKING; MODEL; MIGHT; RUMINATION; MEMORIES; JUDGMENT; BLAME;
D O I
10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00515
中图分类号
B84 [心理学];
学科分类号
04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
A growing body of research suggests that counterfactual thinking after traumatic events is associated with post-traumatic stress reactions. In this study we explored frequency of upward and downward counterfactuals in trauma-exposed individuals, and how trauma-related counterfactuals were represented in terms of vividness. We examined the relationships between vividness and frequency of counterfactual thoughts and post-traumatic stress reactions in two groups who had experienced different types of traumatic exposure, namely survivors and bereaved from the fire on the ferry Scandinavian Star in 1990. Even after 26 years, both survivors and bereaved reported that they currently entertained thoughts about what could have happened during the fire on Scandinavian Star. Survivors reported more downward counterfactuals than the bereaved, whereas the bereaved reported more upward counterfactuals than the survivors did. Vividness of counterfactual thoughts, as well as reported frequency of upward and downward counterfactuals, were associated with post-traumatic stress reactions. Our results suggest that both upward and downward counterfactuals can be harmful, and that vivid counterfactuals about a traumatic event might play a similar role in post-traumatic stress as trauma memories. Therefore, traumatized individuals who entertain counterfactual thoughts may benefit from interventions that target these thoughts specifically.
引用
收藏
页数:8
相关论文
共 51 条
[1]  
[Anonymous], 1999, PERSONALITY SOCIAL P, DOI [DOI 10.1177/, DOI 10.1177/0146167299025005002]
[2]  
[Anonymous], 2013, Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
[3]   Worrying and rumination are both associated with reduced cognitive control [J].
Beckwe, Mieke ;
Deroost, Natacha ;
Koster, Ernst H. W. ;
De Lissnyder, Evi ;
De Raedt, Rudi .
PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH-PSYCHOLOGISCHE FORSCHUNG, 2014, 78 (05) :651-660
[4]   Splintered memories or vivid landmarks? Qualities and organization of traumatic memories with and without PTSD [J].
Berntsen, D ;
Willert, M ;
Rubin, DC .
APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY, 2003, 17 (06) :675-693
[5]   The Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5): Development and Initial Psychometric Evaluation [J].
Blevins, Christy A. ;
Weathers, Frank W. ;
Davis, Margaret T. ;
Witte, Tracy K. ;
Domino, Jessica L. .
JOURNAL OF TRAUMATIC STRESS, 2015, 28 (06) :489-498
[6]  
Blix I, 2016, APPL COGNITIVE PSYCH, V30, P983, DOI 10.1002/acp.3289
[7]   Counterfactual thinking, blame assignment, and well-being in rape victims [J].
Branscombe, NR ;
Wohl, MJA ;
Owen, S ;
Allison, JA ;
N'gbala, A .
BASIC AND APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2003, 25 (04) :265-273
[8]   A dual representation theory of posttraumatic stress disorder [J].
Brewin, CR ;
Dalgleish, T ;
Joseph, S .
PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW, 1996, 103 (04) :670-686
[9]  
Browne M.W., 1993, SOCIOL METHOD RES, P445
[10]   Counterfactual thinking and psychological distress following recurrent miscarriage [J].
Callander, Gemma ;
Brown, Gary P. .
JOURNAL OF REPRODUCTIVE AND INFANT PSYCHOLOGY, 2007, 25 (01) :51-65