Habituation and the reinforcing effectiveness of visual stimuli

被引:23
作者
Lloyd, David R. [1 ,2 ]
Gancarz, Amy M. [1 ,2 ]
Ashrafioun, Lisham [1 ,2 ]
Kausch, Michael A. [1 ]
Richards, Jerry B. [1 ]
机构
[1] SUNY Buffalo, Res Inst Addict, Buffalo, NY 14203 USA
[2] SUNY Buffalo, Dept Psychol, Buffalo, NY 14260 USA
关键词
Learning; Novelty; Operant conditioning; Rat; Sensory reinforcement; NOVELTY SEEKING; LIGHT-ONSET; NICOTINE; SATIATION; BEHAVIOR; INTERVAL; AROUSAL;
D O I
10.1016/j.beproc.2012.07.007
中图分类号
B84 [心理学];
学科分类号
04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
The term "sensory reinforcer" has been used to refer to sensory stimuli (e.g. light onset) that are primary reinforcers in order to differentiate them from other more biologically important primary reinforcers (e.g. food and water). Acquisition of snout poke responding for a visual stimulus (5s light onset) with fixed ratio 1 (FR 1), variable-interval 1 min (VI 1 min), or variable-interval 6 min (VI 6 min) schedules of reinforcement was tested in three groups of rats (n = 8/group). The VI 6 min schedule of reinforcement produced a higher response rate than the FR 1 or VI 1 min schedules of visual stimulus reinforcement. One explanation for greater responding on the VI 6 min schedule relative to the FR 1 and VI 1 min schedules is that the reinforcing effectiveness of light onset habituated more rapidly in the FR 1 and VI 1 min groups as compared to the VI 6 min group. The inverse relationship between response rate and the rate of visual stimulus reinforcement is opposite to results from studies with biologically important reinforcers which indicate a positive relationship between response and reinforcement rate. Rapid habituation of reinforcing effectiveness may be a fundamental characteristic of sensory reinforcers that differentiates them from biologically important reinforcers, which are required to maintain homeostatic balance. (C) 2012 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
引用
收藏
页码:184 / 191
页数:8
相关论文
共 43 条
[1]   Habituation may contribute to within-session decreases in responding under high-rate schedules of reinforcement [J].
Aoyama, K ;
McSweeney, FK .
ANIMAL LEARNING & BEHAVIOR, 2001, 29 (01) :79-91
[2]   Psychobiology of novelty seeking and drug seeking behavior [J].
Bardo, MT ;
Donohew, RL ;
Harrington, NG .
BEHAVIOURAL BRAIN RESEARCH, 1996, 77 (1-2) :23-43
[3]   STIMULUS COMPLEXITY AND SENSORY REINFORCEMENT [J].
BARNES, GW ;
BARON, A .
JOURNAL OF COMPARATIVE AND PHYSIOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY, 1961, 54 (04) :466-&
[5]  
Berlyne D.E., 1969, REINFORCEMENT BEHAV
[6]   NOVELTY AROUSAL AND REINFORCEMENT OF DIVERSIVE EXPLORATION IN RAT [J].
BERLYNE, DE ;
KOENIG, IDV ;
HIROTA, T .
JOURNAL OF COMPARATIVE AND PHYSIOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY, 1966, 62 (02) :222-&
[7]   THE AROUSAL AND SATIATION OF PERCEPTUAL CURIOSITY IN THE RAT [J].
BERLYNE, DE .
JOURNAL OF COMPARATIVE AND PHYSIOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY, 1955, 48 (04) :238-246
[8]   Novelty seeking and reward: Implications for the study of high-risk behaviors [J].
Bevins, RA .
CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE, 2001, 10 (06) :189-193
[9]   EFFECTS OF CHANGING INTERSTIMULUS-INTERVAL DURING HABITUATION IN CAENORHABDITIS-ELEGANS [J].
BROSTER, BS ;
RANKIN, CH .
BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE, 1994, 108 (06) :1019-1029
[10]   The Role of Nicotine in Smoking: A Dual-Reinforcement Model [J].
Caggiula, Anthony R. ;
Donny, Eric C. ;
Palmatier, Matthew I. ;
Liu, Xiu ;
Chaudhri, Nadia ;
Sved, Alan F. .
MOTIVATIONAL IMPACT OF NICOTINE AND ITS ROLE IN TOBACCO USE, 2009, 55 :91-109