Long-Term Language Development in Children With Early Simultaneous Bilateral Cochlear Implants

被引:63
作者
Wie, Ona Bo [1 ,2 ]
Torkildsen, Janne von Koss [1 ]
Schauber, Stefan [3 ]
Busch, Tobias [1 ]
Litovsky, Ruth [4 ]
机构
[1] Univ Oslo, Dept Special Needs Educ, POB 1072 Blindern, N-0316 Oslo, Norway
[2] Oslo Univ Hosp, Dept Otolaryngol, Oslo, Norway
[3] Univ Oslo, Ctr Hlth Sci Educ, Fac Med, Oslo, Norway
[4] Univ Wisconsin Madison, Dept Commun Sci & Disorders, Madison, WI USA
关键词
Bilateral implantation; Child; Cochlear implant; Language; Language development; SPEECH-PERCEPTION; DEAF-CHILDREN; YOUNG-CHILDREN; SPEAKING CHILDREN; NARRATIVE SKILLS; HEARING-LOSS; AGE; VOCABULARY; OUTCOMES; RECOGNITION;
D O I
10.1097/AUD.0000000000000851
中图分类号
R36 [病理学]; R76 [耳鼻咽喉科学];
学科分类号
100104 ; 100213 ;
摘要
Objectives: This longitudinal study followed the language development of children who received the combination of early (5 to 18 months) and simultaneous bilateral cochlear implants (CIs) throughout the first 6 years after implantation. It examined the trajectories of their language development and identified factors associated with language outcomes. Design: Participants were 21 Norwegian children who received bilateral CIs between the ages of 5 and 18 mo and 21 children with normal hearing (NH) who were matched to the children with CIs on age, sex, and maternal education. The language skills of these two groups were compared at 10 time points (3, 6, 9, 12, 18, 24, 36, 48, 60, and 72 months after implantation) using parent reports and standardized measures of general language skills, vocabulary, and grammar. In addition, assessments were made of the effects of age at CI activation, speech recognition abilities, and mothers' education on language outcomes 6 years after implantation. Results: During the first 4 years after implantation, the gap in general expressive and receptive language abilities between children with CIs and children with NH gradually closed. While at the initial five to six assessments (3 to 36 months after implantation), significant differences between children with CIs and children with NH were observed; at 4 years after implantation, there were no longer any significant group differences in general language skills and most children with CIs achieved scores within 1 SD of the tests' normative means. From 2 to 3 years after implantation onward, expressive vocabulary and receptive grammar skills of children with CIs were similar to those of the reference group. However, from 4 years after implantation until the end of the observation period, 6 years after implantation, expressive grammar skills of children with CIs were lower than those of children with NH. In addition, a gap in receptive vocabulary appeared and grew increasingly larger from 4 to 6 years postimplantation. At the final assessment, the children with CIs had an average receptive vocabulary score around 1 SD below the normative mean. Regression analysis indicated that the children's language outcomes at 6 years after implantation were related to their speech recognition skills, age at CI activation, and maternal education. Conclusions: In the first 4 years after implantation, the language performance of children with CIs became increasingly similar to that of their NH peers. However, between 4 and 6 years after implantation, there were indications of challenges with certain aspects of language, specifically receptive vocabulary and expressive grammar. Because these challenges first appeared after the 4-year assessment, the findings underline the importance of long-term language intervention to increase the chances of a continued language development comparable to that of NH peers. They also indicate that there is a need for comprehensive longitudinal studies of the language development of children with CIs beyond 4 years after implantation.
引用
收藏
页码:1294 / 1305
页数:12
相关论文
共 84 条
  • [1] [Anonymous], 1995, MULLEN SCALES EARLY
  • [2] [Anonymous], 2004, COLOURED PROGR MATRI
  • [3] [Anonymous], 2000, Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence
  • [4] Bilateral versus unilateral cochlear implants in children: Speech recognition, sound localization, and parental reports
    Asp, Filip
    Maki-Torkko, Elina
    Karltorp, Eva
    Harder, Henrik
    Hergils, Leif
    Eskilsson, Gunnar
    Stenfelt, Stefan
    [J]. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF AUDIOLOGY, 2012, 51 (11) : 817 - 832
  • [5] Bishop D., 2003, Test of Reception of Grammar2 (T.R.O.G, Swedish version, 2009
  • [6] Black Jane, 2014, Cochlear Implants Int, V15, P62, DOI 10.1179/1754762813Y.0000000045
  • [7] Relationships among speech perception, production, language, hearing loss, and age in children with impaired hearing
    Blamey, PJ
    Sarant, JZ
    Paatsch, LE
    Barry, JG
    Bow, CP
    Wales, RJ
    Wright, M
    Psarros, C
    Rattigan, K
    Tooher, R
    [J]. JOURNAL OF SPEECH LANGUAGE AND HEARING RESEARCH, 2001, 44 (02): : 264 - 285
  • [8] Six-year-olds' learning of novel words through addressed and overheard speech
    Bodere, Anneleen
    Jaspaert, Koen
    [J]. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE, 2017, 44 (05) : 1163 - 1191
  • [9] Expressive vocabulary, morphology, syntax and narrative skills in profoundly deaf children after early cochlear implantation
    Boons, Tinne
    De Raeve, Leo
    Langereis, Margreet
    Peeraer, Louis
    Wouters, Jan
    van Wieringen, Astrid
    [J]. RESEARCH IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES, 2013, 34 (06) : 2008 - 2022
  • [10] Predictors of Spoken Language Development Following Pediatric Cochlear Implantation
    Boons, Tinne
    Brokx, Jan P. L.
    Dhooge, Ingeborg
    Frijns, Johan H. M.
    Peeraer, Louis
    Vermeulen, Anneke
    Wouters, Jan
    van Wieringen, Astrid
    [J]. EAR AND HEARING, 2012, 33 (05) : 627 - 639