Failures of Perception in the Low-Prevalence Effect: Evidence From Active and Passive Visual Search

被引:64
作者
Hout, Michael C. [1 ]
Walenchok, Stephen C. [2 ]
Goldinger, Stephen D. [2 ]
Wolfe, Jeremy M. [3 ,4 ]
机构
[1] New Mexico State Univ, Dept Psychol, Las Cruces, NM 88003 USA
[2] Arizona State Univ, Dept Psychol, Tempe, AZ 85287 USA
[3] Brigham & Womens Hosp, Cambridge, MA USA
[4] Harvard Univ, Sch Med, Dept Ophthalmol, Cambridge, MA 02138 USA
基金
美国国家卫生研究院;
关键词
visual search; eye movements; target prevalence; LOW TARGET PREVALENCE; LONG-TERM-MEMORY; HYBRID SEARCH; EXPECTATIONS; ATTENTION; COST; RARE; SPECIFICITY; CAPACITY; GUIDANCE;
D O I
10.1037/xhp0000053
中图分类号
B84 [心理学];
学科分类号
04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
In visual search, rare targets are missed disproportionately often. This low-prevalence effect (LPE) is a robust problem with demonstrable societal consequences. What is the source of the LPE? Is it a perceptual bias against rare targets or a later process, such as premature search termination or motor response errors? In 4 experiments, we examined the LPE using standard visual search (with eye tracking) and 2 variants of rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) in which observers made present/absent decisions after sequences ended. In all experiments, observers looked for 2 target categories (teddy bear and butterfly) simultaneously. To minimize simple motor errors, caused by repetitive absent responses, we held overall target prevalence at 50%, with 1 low-prevalence and 1 high-prevalence target type. Across conditions, observers either searched for targets among other real-world objects or searched for specific bears or butterflies among within-category distractors. We report 4 main results: (a) In standard search, high-prevalence targets were found more quickly and accurately than low-prevalence targets. (b) The LPE persisted in RSVP search, even though observers never terminated search on their own. (c) Eye-tracking analyses showed that high-prevalence targets elicited better attentional guidance and faster perceptual decisions. And (d) even when observers looked directly at low-prevalence targets, they often (12%-34% of trials) failed to detect them. These results strongly argue that low-prevalence misses represent failures of perception when early search termination or motor errors are controlled.
引用
收藏
页码:977 / 994
页数:18
相关论文
共 61 条
[1]   Simultaneous Control of Attention by Multiple Working Memory Representations [J].
Beck, Valerie M. ;
Hollingworth, Andrew ;
Luck, Steven J. .
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE, 2012, 23 (08) :887-898
[2]   Bumblebees Learn to Forage like Bayesians [J].
Biernaskie, Jay M. ;
Walker, Steven C. ;
Gegear, Robert J. .
AMERICAN NATURALIST, 2009, 174 (03) :413-423
[3]   Hybrid search in context: How to search for vegetables in the produce section and cereal in the cereal aisle [J].
Boettcher, Sage E. P. ;
Drew, Trafton ;
Wolfe, Jeremy M. .
VISUAL COGNITION, 2013, 21 (06) :678-682
[4]   Visual long-term memory has a massive storage capacity for object details [J].
Brady, Timothy F. ;
Konkle, Talia ;
Alvarez, George A. ;
Oliva, Aude .
PROCEEDINGS OF THE NATIONAL ACADEMY OF SCIENCES OF THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA, 2008, 105 (38) :14325-14329
[5]   Task demands determine the specificity of the search template [J].
Bravo, Mary J. ;
Farid, Hany .
ATTENTION PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS, 2012, 74 (01) :124-131
[6]   The specificity of the search template [J].
Bravo, Mary J. ;
Farid, Hany .
JOURNAL OF VISION, 2009, 9 (01)
[7]  
Breast Cancer Surveillance Consortium, 2009, CANC RAT 1000 EX CAN
[8]   A Bayesian Optimal Foraging Model of Human Visual Search [J].
Cain, Matthew S. ;
Vul, Edward ;
Clark, Kait ;
Mitroff, Stephen R. .
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE, 2012, 23 (09) :1047-1054
[9]   OPTIMAL FORAGING, MARGINAL VALUE THEOREM [J].
CHARNOV, EL .
THEORETICAL POPULATION BIOLOGY, 1976, 9 (02) :129-136
[10]   Just say no: How are visual searches terminated when there is no target present? [J].
Chun, MM ;
Wolfe, JM .
COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY, 1996, 30 (01) :39-78