Combining speed and accuracy to control for speed-accuracy trade-offs(?)

被引:249
作者
Liesefeld, Heinrich Rene [1 ,2 ]
Janczyk, Markus [3 ]
机构
[1] Ludwig Maximilians Univ Munchen, Dept Psychol, Leopoldstr 13, D-80802 Munich, Germany
[2] Ludwig Maximilians Univ Munchen, Grad Sch Syst Neurosci, Leopoldstr 13, D-80802 Munich, Germany
[3] Eberhard Karls Univ Tubingen, Dept Psychol, Tubingen, Germany
关键词
Speed-accuracy trade-off; Integration of errors and RTs; Integrated scoring; Task instructions; Performanc strategies; Methods in experimental psychology; VISUAL WORKING-MEMORY; DIFFUSION-MODEL; RESPONSE-TIME; AUDIOVISUAL INTEGRATION; VISUOTACTILE SPACE; TERM-MEMORY; CAPACITY; EZ; ADJUSTMENTS; PERFORMANCE;
D O I
10.3758/s13428-018-1076-x
中图分类号
B841 [心理学研究方法];
学科分类号
040201 ;
摘要
In psychological experiments, participants are typically instructed to respond as fast as possible without sacrificing accuracy. How they interpret this instruction and, consequently, which speed-accuracy trade-off they choose might vary between experiments, between participants, and between conditions. Consequently, experimental effects can appear unpredictably in either RTs or error rates (i.e., accuracy). Even more problematic, spurious effects might emerge that are actually due only to differential speed-accuracy trade-offs. An often-suggested solution is the inverse efficiency score (IES; Townsend & Ashby, 1983), which combines speed and accuracy into a single score. Alternatives are the rate-correct score (RCS; Woltz & Was, 2006) and the linear-integrated speed-accuracy score (LISAS; Vandierendonck, 2017, 2018). We report analyses on simulated data generated with the standard diffusion model (Ratcliff, 1978) showing that IES, RCS, and LISAS put unequal weights on speed and accuracy, depending on the accuracy level, and that these measures are actually very sensitive to speed-accuracy trade-offs. These findings stand in contrast to a fourth alternative, the balanced integration score (BIS; Liesefeld, Fu, & Zimmer, 2015), which was devised to integrate speed and accuracy with equal weights. Although all of the measures maintain "real" effects, only BIS is relatively insensitive to speed-accuracy trade-offs.
引用
收藏
页码:40 / 60
页数:21
相关论文
共 75 条
[1]   RELATIONS BETWEEN COVERT ORIENTING AND FILTERING IN THE DEVELOPMENT OF VISUAL-ATTENTION [J].
AKHTAR, N ;
ENNS, JT .
JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL CHILD PSYCHOLOGY, 1989, 48 (02) :315-334
[2]   The capacity of visual short-term memory is set both by visual information load and by number of objects [J].
Alvarez, GA ;
Cavanagh, P .
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE, 2004, 15 (02) :106-111
[3]   Acquisition of decision making criteria: reward rate ultimately beats accuracy [J].
Balci, Fuat ;
Simen, Patrick ;
Niyogi, Ritwik ;
Saxe, Andrew ;
Hughes, Jessica A. ;
Holmes, Philip ;
Cohen, Jonathan D. .
ATTENTION PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS, 2011, 73 (02) :640-657
[4]   Conflict monitoring and cognitive control [J].
Botvinick, MM ;
Braver, TS ;
Barch, DM ;
Carter, CS ;
Cohen, JD .
PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW, 2001, 108 (03) :624-652
[5]   The simplest complete model of choice response time: Linear ballistic accumulation [J].
Brown, Scott D. ;
Heathcote, Andrew .
COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY, 2008, 57 (03) :153-178
[6]   COMBINING SPEED AND ACCURACY IN COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY: IS THE INVERSE EFFICIENCY SCORE (IES) A BETTER DEPENDENT VARIABLE THAN THE MEAN REACTION TIME (RT) AND THE PERCENTAGE OF ERRORS (PE)? [J].
Bruyer, Raymond ;
Brysbaert, Marc .
PSYCHOLOGICA BELGICA, 2011, 51 (01) :5-13
[7]   TRANSFORMATIONS FOR WITHIN-SUBJECT DESIGNS - A MONTE-CARLO INVESTIGATION [J].
BUSH, LK ;
HESS, U ;
WOLFORD, G .
PSYCHOLOGICAL BULLETIN, 1993, 113 (03) :566-579
[8]  
Cohen JW., 1988, STAT POWER ANAL BEHA, DOI 10.4324/9780203771587
[9]   Audio-visual integration of emotion expression [J].
Collignon, Olivier ;
Girard, Simon ;
Gosselin, Frederic ;
Roy, Sylvain ;
Saint-Amour, Dave ;
Lassonde, Maryse ;
Lepore, Franco .
BRAIN RESEARCH, 2008, 1242 :126-135
[10]   Drawing conclusions from choice response time models: A tutorial using the linear ballistic accumulator [J].
Donkin, Chris ;
Brown, Scott ;
Heathcote, Andrew .
JOURNAL OF MATHEMATICAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2011, 55 (02) :140-151