Navigating in a three-dimensional world

被引:96
|
作者
Jeffery, Kathryn J. [1 ]
Jovalekic, Aleksandar [2 ]
Verriotis, Madeleine [3 ]
Hayman, Robin [4 ]
机构
[1] UCL, Dept Cognit Perceptual & Brain Sci, Div Psychol & Language Sci, London WC1H 0AP, England
[2] Univ Zurich, Inst Neuroinformat, CH-8057 Zurich, Switzerland
[3] UCL, Dept Neurosci Physiol & Pharmacol, London WC1E 6BT, England
[4] Inst Cognit Neurosci, London WC1N 3AR, England
基金
英国医学研究理事会; 英国惠康基金; 英国生物技术与生命科学研究理事会;
关键词
ethology; grid cells; head direction cells; hippocampus; navigation; neural encoding; place cells; spatial cognition; three-dimensional; HEAD-DIRECTION CELLS; FREELY MOVING RATS; GEOGRAPHICAL SLANT; DESERT ANTS; PLACE CELLS; SPATIAL REPRESENTATION; PATH-INTEGRATION; STINGLESS BEE; SPACE; SLOPE;
D O I
10.1017/S0140525X12002476
中图分类号
B84 [心理学];
学科分类号
04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
The study of spatial cognition has provided considerable insight into how animals (including humans) navigate on the horizontal plane. However, the real world is three-dimensional, having a complex topography including both horizontal and vertical features, which presents additional challenges for representation and navigation. The present article reviews the emerging behavioral and neurobiological literature on spatial cognition in non-horizontal environments. We suggest that three-dimensional spaces are represented in a quasiplanar fashion, with space in the plane of locomotion being computed separately and represented differently from space in the orthogonal axis - a representational structure we have termed "bicoded." We argue that the mammalian spatial representation in surface-travelling animals comprises a mosaic of these locally planar fragments, rather than a fully integrated volumetric map. More generally, this may be true even for species that can move freely in all three dimensions, such as birds and fish. We outline the evidence supporting this view, together with the adaptive advantages of such a scheme.
引用
收藏
页码:523 / 543
页数:21
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [1] Navigating and visualizing three-dimensional data sets
    John, NW
    McCloy, RF
    BRITISH JOURNAL OF RADIOLOGY, 2004, 77 : S108 - S113
  • [2] Navigating through a volumetric world does not imply needing a full three-dimensional representation
    Carbon, Claus-Christian
    Hesslinger, Vera M.
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES, 2013, 36 (05) : 547 - 548
  • [3] The three-dimensional world of the molecules
    Koch, Oliver
    NACHRICHTEN AUS DER CHEMIE, 2009, 57 (09) : 894 - 896
  • [4] Vision in our three-dimensional world
    Parker, Andrew J.
    PHILOSOPHICAL TRANSACTIONS OF THE ROYAL SOCIETY B-BIOLOGICAL SCIENCES, 2016, 371 (1697)
  • [5] A moving observer in a three-dimensional world
    Glennerster, Andrew
    PHILOSOPHICAL TRANSACTIONS OF THE ROYAL SOCIETY B-BIOLOGICAL SCIENCES, 2016, 371 (1697)
  • [6] MolSurfer: two-dimensional maps for navigating three-dimensional structures of proteins
    Gabdoulline, RR
    Wade, RC
    Walther, D
    TRENDS IN BIOCHEMICAL SCIENCES, 1999, 24 (07) : 285 - 287
  • [7] Taking Command of Three-Dimensional Stitching Artifacts: From an Annoyance to an Easy Tool for Navigating Three-Dimensional Transesophageal Echocardiography
    Maidman, Samuel D.
    Bamira, Daniel
    Ro, Richard
    Vainrib, Alan F.
    Saric, Muhamed
    JOURNAL OF THE AMERICAN SOCIETY OF ECHOCARDIOGRAPHY, 2023, 36 (01) : 105 - 110
  • [8] THREE-DIMENSIONAL COMPUTATIONAL STRUCTURES AND THE REAL WORLD
    Lonsing, Werner
    Anders, Peter
    PROCEEDINGS OF THE 16TH INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE ON COMPUTER-AIDED ARCHITECTURAL DESIGN RESEARCH IN ASIA (CAADRIA 2011): CIRCUIT BENDING, BREAKING AND MENDING, 2011, : 209 - 218
  • [9] An examination of interactions in a three-dimensional virtual world
    Yilmaz, Rabia M.
    Baydas, Ozlem
    Karakus, Turkan
    Goktas, Yuksel
    COMPUTERS & EDUCATION, 2015, 88 : 256 - 267
  • [10] Artificial enactive inference in three-dimensional world
    Georgeon, Olivier L.
    Lurie, David
    Robertson, Paul
    Cognitive Systems Research, 2024, 86