Inventing a solution and studying a worked solution prepare differently for learning from direct instruction

被引:99
作者
Glogger-Frey, Inga [1 ]
Fleischer, Corinna [1 ]
Grueny, Lisa [1 ]
Kappich, Julian [1 ]
Renkl, Alexander [1 ]
机构
[1] Univ Freiburg, Dept Educ & Dev Psychol, D-79085 Freiburg, Germany
关键词
Invention activities; Worked examples; Teacher education; Physics education; Preparation for learning; SELF-EFFICACY; PRIOR KNOWLEDGE; STUDENTS; EXAMPLES; EXPLANATIONS; MOTIVATION; FAILURE; ACQUISITION; STRATEGIES; EFFICIENCY;
D O I
10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.05.001
中图分类号
G40 [教育学];
学科分类号
040101 ; 120403 ;
摘要
Solving an open problem as proposed by inventing and productive failure approaches has been shown to prepare learners effectively for subsequent direct instruction. Inventing can raise awareness of knowledge gaps (cognitive) as well as increase curiosity about and interest in the learning contents (motivational effects). However, studying the problem with a worked solution can have different cognitive and motivational advantages. In two experiments in quite different domains and settings (N-1 = 42; N-2 = 40), we tested to what extent working on an open problem (inventing) as opposed to studying a worked solution of the same problem better prepares (1) student teachers for learning-strategy evaluation, and (2) 8th-graders for learning about ratios in physics. Transfer was better supported by a worked solution. Mediators were the (self-regulated) learning time on most relevant learning contents (Experiment 1) and self-efficacy (Experiment 2). Inventing, however, increased knowledge-gap experience as well as (in Experiment 1) interest and curiosity. The stable transfer effect in two different domains and settings raises interesting questions for further research that had not been systematically investigated so far. (C) 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
引用
收藏
页码:72 / 87
页数:16
相关论文
共 95 条
[1]  
[Anonymous], 2006, Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents
[2]   SELF-EFFICACY - TOWARD A UNIFYING THEORY OF BEHAVIORAL CHANGE [J].
BANDURA, A .
PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW, 1977, 84 (02) :191-215
[3]  
Bandura A, 1997, Self -Efficacy: The Exercise of Control
[4]   When and where do we apply what we learn? A taxonomy for far transfer [J].
Barnett, SM ;
Ceci, SJ .
PSYCHOLOGICAL BULLETIN, 2002, 128 (04) :612-637
[5]  
Begolli K.N., 2013, Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, P1863
[6]   Motivation and Transfer: The Role of Mastery-Approach Goals in Preparation for Future Learning [J].
Belenky, Daniel M. ;
Nokes-Malach, Timothy J. .
JOURNAL OF THE LEARNING SCIENCES, 2012, 21 (03) :399-432
[7]   Do learning protocols support learning strategies and outcomes?: The role of cognitive and metacognitive prompts [J].
Berthold, Kirsten ;
Nueckles, Matthias ;
Renkl, Alexander .
LEARNING AND INSTRUCTION, 2007, 17 (05) :564-577
[8]   How to Foster Active Processing of Explanations in Instructional Communication [J].
Berthold, Kirsten ;
Renkl, Alexander .
EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW, 2010, 22 (01) :25-40
[9]  
Biggs J.B., 1982, Evaluating the quality of learning: The SOLO taxonomy (Structure of the Observed Learning Outcome)
[10]  
Bromme R., 2004, The international encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences, DOI DOI 10.1016/B0-08-043076-7/02447-5