Randomized or fixed order for studies of behavioral syndromes?

被引:93
作者
Bell, Alison [1 ]
机构
[1] Univ Illinois, Sch Integrat Biol, Urbana, IL 61801 USA
基金
美国国家科学基金会;
关键词
behavioral syndromes; carryover effect; coping styles; experimental design; personality; randomization; temperament; FITNESS CONSEQUENCES; AGGRESSIVE-BEHAVIOR; SEX MATTERS; PERSONALITY; BOLDNESS; EVOLUTION; ECOLOGY; SUNFISH; TRAITS; SPIDER;
D O I
10.1093/beheco/ars148
中图分类号
B84 [心理学]; C [社会科学总论]; Q98 [人类学];
学科分类号
03 ; 0303 ; 030303 ; 04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
There is a growing interest among behavioral ecologists in behavioral syndromes and animal personality. Studies of behavioral syndromes repeatedly measure the same individuals to quantify within-individual consistency and between-individual variation in behavior. Often these studies measure behavior in different contexts or in different behavioral assays to determine whether individual differences in behavior in one context are related to behavior in other contexts, that is, if there is a behavioral syndrome. For studies of behavioral syndromes, there is not universal agreement about whether it is preferable to randomize the order of different assays or to administer them in a fixed order. Here, I articulate the advantages and disadvantages of testing in a randomized or fixed order and offer some recommendations according to the goals and power of the experiment. In general, studies using within-subjects designs that are primarily interested in mean-level differences between treatments should randomize the order that individuals experience different treatments. Under certain conditions, studies of behavioral syndromes should also administer the assays in a randomized order, but only if the study is sufficiently powerful to statistically account for carryover and period effects. If the experimenter is interested in behavioral syndromes that are caused by carryovers, it is often preferable to test in a fixed order. If the experimenter wants to guard against carryovers, but the experiment is not sufficiently powerful to account for carryover and period effects, then a compromise is to test in a fixed order, but to test individuals in the context that is most likely to affect subsequent behavior last.
引用
收藏
页码:16 / 20
页数:5
相关论文
共 41 条
[1]   Shy trout grow faster: exploring links between personality and fitness-related traits in the wild [J].
Adriaenssens, Bart ;
Johnsson, Jorgen I. .
BEHAVIORAL ECOLOGY, 2011, 22 (01) :135-143
[2]   Variable neuroendocrine responses to ecologically-relevant challenges in sticklebacks [J].
Bell, Alison M. ;
Backstrom, Tobias ;
Huntingford, Felicity A. ;
Pottinger, Tom G. ;
Winberg, Svante .
PHYSIOLOGY & BEHAVIOR, 2007, 91 (01) :15-25
[3]   Behavioral syndrome over the boundaries of life-025EFcarryovers from larvae to adult damselfly [J].
Brodin, Tomas .
BEHAVIORAL ECOLOGY, 2009, 20 (01) :30-37
[4]   Behavioural syndromes at multiple scales in Myrmica ants [J].
Chapman, Ben B. ;
Thain, Harry ;
Coughlin, Jennifer ;
Hughes, William O. H. .
ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2011, 82 (02) :391-397
[5]   Personality traits predict hierarchy rank in male rainbowfish social groups [J].
Colleter, Mathieu ;
Brown, Culum .
ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2011, 81 (06) :1231-1237
[6]   Personality predicts social dominance in female zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata, in a feeding context [J].
David, Morgan ;
Auclair, Yannick ;
Cezilly, Frank .
ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2011, 81 (01) :219-224
[7]  
Díaz-Uriarte R, 2002, ANIM BEHAV, V63, P815
[8]   Behavioural reaction norms: animal personality meets individual plasticity [J].
Dingemanse, Niels J. ;
Kazem, Anahita J. N. ;
Reale, Denis ;
Wright, Jonathan .
TRENDS IN ECOLOGY & EVOLUTION, 2010, 25 (02) :81-89
[9]   Behavioral syndromes: carryover effects, false discovery rates, and a priori hypotheses [J].
Dochtermann, Ned A. .
BEHAVIORAL ECOLOGY, 2010, 21 (03) :437-439
[10]   Behavioral correlations across breeding contexts provide a mechanism for a cost of aggression [J].
Duckworth, Renee A. .
BEHAVIORAL ECOLOGY, 2006, 17 (06) :1011-1019