Emotional responses to prosocial messages increase willingness to self-isolate during the COVID-19 pandemic

被引:140
作者
Heffner, Joseph [1 ]
Vives, Marc-Lluis [1 ]
FeldmanHall, Oriel [1 ,2 ]
机构
[1] Brown Univ, Dept Cognit Linguist Psychol Sci, Providence, RI 02912 USA
[2] Brown Univ, Carney Inst Brain Sci, Providence, RI 02912 USA
关键词
Emotion; COVID-19; Public health messaging; Response efficacy; Persuasion; Extraversion; Neuroticism; COMMUNICATING CLIMATE-CHANGE; INDIVIDUAL-DIFFERENCES; POLICY PREFERENCES; NEGATIVITY BIAS; HEALTH MESSAGES; FEAR APPEALS; METAANALYSIS; NEUROTICISM; RISK; INFORMATION;
D O I
10.1016/j.paid.2020.110420
中图分类号
B84 [心理学];
学科分类号
04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
The COVID-19 pandemic may be one of the greatest modern societal challenges that requires widespread collective action and cooperation. While a handful of actions can help reduce pathogen transmission, one critical behavior is to self-isolate. Public health messages often use persuasive language to change attitudes and behaviors, which can evoke a wide range of negative and positive emotional responses. In a U.S. representative sample (N = 955), we presented two messages that leveraged either threatening or prosocial persuasive language, and measured self-reported emotional reactions and willingness to self-isolate. Although emotional responses to the interventions were highly heterogeneous, personality traits known to be linked with distinct emotional experiences (extraversion and neuroticism) explained significant variance in the arousal response. While results show that both types of appeals increased willingness to self-isolate (Cohen's d = 0.41), compared to the threat message, the efficacy of the prosocial message was more dependent on the magnitude of the evoked emotional response on both arousal and valence dimensions. Together, these results imply that prosocial appeals have the potential to be associated with greater compliance if they evoke highly positive emotional responses.
引用
收藏
页数:8
相关论文
共 84 条
  • [1] Arenas A., 2020, DERIVATION EFFECTIVE, DOI [10.1101/2020.04.06.20054320, DOI 10.1101/2020.04.06.20054320]
  • [2] THE 20-ITEM TORONTO-ALEXITHYMIA-SCALE .1. ITEM SELECTION AND CROSS-VALIDATION OF THE FACTOR STRUCTURE
    BAGBY, RM
    PARKER, JDA
    TAYLOR, GJ
    [J]. JOURNAL OF PSYCHOSOMATIC RESEARCH, 1994, 38 (01) : 23 - 32
  • [3] Goal-directed emotions
    Bagozzi, RP
    Baumgartner, H
    Pieters, R
    [J]. COGNITION & EMOTION, 1998, 12 (01) : 1 - 26
  • [4] Baron J., 1994, THINKING DECIDING, V2nd
  • [5] Beech H., 2020, NEW YORK TIMES
  • [6] Boster F.J., 1984, COMMUNICATION YB, V8, P330, DOI [DOI 10.1080/23808985.1984.11678581, 10.1080/23808985.1984.11678581]
  • [7] Brooks Samantha K, 2020, Lancet, V395, P912, DOI [10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30460-8., 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30460-8]
  • [8] Canipe C., 2020, SOCIAL DISTANCING AM
  • [9] Amygdala response to happy faces as a function of extraversion
    Canli, T
    Sivers, H
    Whitfield, SL
    Gotlib, IH
    Gabrieli, JDE
    [J]. SCIENCE, 2002, 296 (5576) : 2191 - 2191
  • [10] Threat appeals in health communication: messages that elicit fear and enhance perceived efficacy positively impact on young male drivers
    Carey, Rachel N.
    Sarma, Kiran M.
    [J]. BMC PUBLIC HEALTH, 2016, 16