Late-Emerging and Resolving Dyslexia: A Follow-Up Study from Age 3 to 14

被引:58
作者
Torppa, Minna [1 ]
Eklund, Kenneth [1 ]
van Bergen, Elsje [2 ]
Lyytinen, Heikki [1 ]
机构
[1] Univ Jyvaskyla, Jyvaskyla, Finland
[2] Univ Oxford, Oxford, England
基金
芬兰科学院;
关键词
Early identification; Family risk for dyslexia; Late-emerging dyslexia; Reading disabilities; Dyslexia; INDIVIDUAL-DIFFERENCES; READING DEVELOPMENT; LITERACY; RISK; CHILDREN; ENGLISH; DEFICIT; STUDENTS; FLUENCY; READERS;
D O I
10.1007/s10802-015-0003-1
中图分类号
B849 [应用心理学];
学科分类号
040203 ;
摘要
This study focuses on the stability of dyslexia status from Grade 2 to Grade 8 in four groups: (a) no dyslexia in either grade (no-dyslexia, n = 127); (b) no dyslexia in Grade 2 but dyslexia in Grade 8 (late-emerging, n = 18); (c) dyslexia in Grade 2 but not in Grade 8 (resolving, n = 15); and (d) dyslexia in both grades (persistent-dyslexia, n = 22). We examined group differences from age 3.5 to age 14 in (a) reading, vocabulary, phonology, letter knowledge, rapid naming, IQ, verbal memory; (b) familial and environmental risk and supportive factors; and (c) parental skills in reading, phonology, rapid naming, verbal memory, and vocabulary. Our findings showed group differences both in reading and cognitive skills of children as well as their parents. Parental education, book-reading frequency, and children's IQ, however, did not differentiate the groups. The children in the persistent-dyslexia group exhibited widespread language and cognitive deficits across development. Those in the resolving group had problems in language and cognitive skills only prior to school entry. In the late-emerging group, children showed clearly compromised rapid naming. Additionally, their parents had the most severe difficulties in rapid naming, a finding that suggests strong genetic liability. The findings show instability in the diagnosis of dyslexia. The members of the late-emerging group did not have a distinct early cognitive profile, so late-emerging dyslexia appears difficult to predict. Indeed, these children are at risk of not being identified and not receiving required support. This study suggests the need for continued monitoring of children's progress in literacy after the early school years.
引用
收藏
页码:1389 / 1401
页数:13
相关论文
共 43 条
[1]  
[Anonymous], 2010, PISA 2009 RESULTS LE, VIII, DOI DOI 10.1787/9789264083943-EN
[2]  
[Anonymous], 2010, PISA 2009 Results: What Students Know and Can Do - Student Performance in Reading, Mathematics and Science, VI, DOI DOI 10.1787/9789264091450-EN
[3]   Learning to read: English in comparison to six more regular orthographies [J].
Aro, M ;
Wimmer, H .
APPLIED PSYCHOLINGUISTICS, 2003, 24 (04) :621-635
[4]  
Brown Waechse J. S., 2011, J LEARNING DISABILIT, V44, P296, DOI [10.1177/ 0022219410392048, DOI 10.1177/0022219410392048]
[5]   Prevalence and Nature of Late-Emerging Poor Readers [J].
Catts, Hugh W. ;
Compton, Donald ;
Tomblin, J. Bruce ;
Bridges, Mindy Sittner .
JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2012, 104 (01) :166-181
[6]   Tracking children who fly below the radar: Latent transition modeling of students with late-emerging reading disability [J].
Compton, Donald L. ;
Fuchs, Douglas ;
Fuchs, Lynn S. ;
Elleman, Amy M. ;
Gilbert, Jennifer K. .
LEARNING AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES, 2008, 18 (03) :329-337
[7]  
de Jong P.F., 2002, SCI STUD READ, V6, P51, DOI [10.1207/S1532799XSSR060103, DOI 10.1207/S1532799XSSR060103]
[8]   Developmental changes in the manifestation of a phonological deficit in dyslexic children learning to read a regular orthography [J].
de Jong, PF ;
van der Leij, A .
JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2003, 95 (01) :22-40
[9]  
Denckla M. B., 1974, CORTEX, V10, P471
[10]   Literacy Skill Development of Children With Familial Risk for Dyslexia Through Grades 2, 3, and 8 [J].
Eklund, Kenneth ;
Torppa, Minna ;
Aro, Mikko ;
Leppanen, Paavo H. T. ;
Lyytinen, Heikki .
JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2015, 107 (01) :126-140