Temporal recalibration of motor and visual potentials in lag adaptation in voluntary movement

被引:12
作者
Cai, Chang [1 ,2 ,3 ]
Ogawa, Kenji [1 ,4 ]
Kochiyama, Takanori [5 ]
Tanaka, Hirokazu [6 ]
Imamizu, Hiroshi [1 ,2 ,3 ,7 ]
机构
[1] Adv Telecommun Res Inst Int, Cognit Mech Labs, Kyoto 6190288, Japan
[2] Natl Inst Informat & Commun Technol, Ctr Informat & Neural Networks, Suita, Osaka 5650871, Japan
[3] Osaka Univ, Suita, Osaka 5650871, Japan
[4] Hokkaido Univ, Grad Sch Letters, Dept Psychol, Sapporo, Hokkaido 0600810, Japan
[5] ATR Promot, Brain Activ Imaging Ctr, Kyoto 6190288, Japan
[6] Japan Adv Inst Sci & Technol, Sch Informat Sci, Nomi, Ishikawa 9231211, Japan
[7] Univ Tokyo, Grad Sch Humanities & Sociol, Dept Psychol, Tokyo 1130033, Japan
关键词
Lag adaptation; Readiness potential; Visually evoked potential; Voluntary movement; NEAR-INFRARED SPECTROSCOPY; CONSCIOUS INTENTION; EVOKED-POTENTIALS; CORTICAL CURRENTS; INTERNAL-MODELS; CORTEX; ATTENTION; PARIETAL; ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAPHY; RECONSTRUCTION;
D O I
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.02.015
中图分类号
Q189 [神经科学];
学科分类号
071006 ;
摘要
Adaptively recalibrating motor-sensory asynchrony is critical for animals to perceive self-produced action consequences. It is controversial whether motor-or sensory-related neural circuits recalibrate this asynchrony. By combining magnetoencephalography (MEG) and functional MRI (fMRI), we investigate the temporal changes in brain activities caused by repeated exposure to a 150-ms delay inserted between a button-press action and a subsequent flash. We found that readiness potentials significantly shift later in the motor system, especially in parietal regions (average: 219.9 ms), while visually evoked potentials significantly shift earlier in occipital regions (average: 49.7 ms) in the delay condition compared to the no-delay condition. Moreover, the shift in readiness potentials, but not in visually evoked potentials, was significantly correlated with the psychophysical measure of motor-sensory adaptation. These results suggest that although both motor and sensory processes contribute to the recalibration, the motor process plays the major role, given the magnitudes of shift and the correlation with the psychophysical measure.
引用
收藏
页码:654 / 662
页数:9
相关论文
共 48 条
[1]   Cortical current source estimation from electroencephalography in combination with near-infrared spectroscopy as a hierarchical prior [J].
Aihara, Takatsugu ;
Takeda, Yusuke ;
Takeda, Kotaro ;
Yasuda, Wataru ;
Sato, Takanori ;
Otaka, Yohei ;
Hanakawa, Takashi ;
Honda, Manabu ;
Liu, Meigen ;
Kawato, Mitsuo ;
Sato, Masa-aki ;
Osu, Rieko .
NEUROIMAGE, 2012, 59 (04) :4006-4021
[2]   Self-awareness and action [J].
Blakemore, SJ ;
Frith, C .
CURRENT OPINION IN NEUROBIOLOGY, 2003, 13 (02) :219-224
[3]   A neural model for temporal order judgments and their active recalibration: a common mechanism for space and time? [J].
Cai, Mingbo ;
Stetson, Chess ;
Eagleman, David M. .
FRONTIERS IN PSYCHOLOGY, 2012, 3
[4]   VISUAL EVOKED-POTENTIALS AND POSITRON EMISSION TOMOGRAPHIC MAPPING OF REGIONAL CEREBRAL BLOOD-FLOW AND CEREBRAL METABOLISM - CAN THE NEURONAL POTENTIAL GENERATORS BE VISUALIZED [J].
CELESIA, GG ;
POLCYN, RD ;
HOLDEN, JE ;
NICKLES, RJ ;
GATLEY, JS ;
KOEPPE, RA .
ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAPHY AND CLINICAL NEUROPHYSIOLOGY, 1982, 54 (03) :243-256
[5]   Movement Intention After Parietal Cortex Stimulation in Humans [J].
Desmurget, Michel ;
Reilly, Karen T. ;
Richard, Nathalie ;
Szathmari, Alexandru ;
Mottolese, Carmine ;
Sirigu, Angela .
SCIENCE, 2009, 324 (5928) :811-813
[6]   Recalibration of multisensory simultaneity: Cross-modal transfer coincides with a change in perceptual latency [J].
Di Luca, Massimiliano ;
Machulla, Tonja-Katrin ;
Ernst, Marc O. .
JOURNAL OF VISION, 2009, 9 (12) :1-16
[7]   Cortical sources of the early components of the visual evoked potential [J].
Di Russo, F ;
Martínez, A ;
Sereno, MI ;
Pitzalis, S ;
Hillyard, SA .
HUMAN BRAIN MAPPING, 2002, 15 (02) :95-111
[8]  
Flanagan JR, 1997, J NEUROSCI, V17, P1519
[9]  
Frith C.D., 2014, The cognitive neuropsychology of schizophrenia
[10]   Conscious intention and motor cognition [J].
Haggard, P .
TRENDS IN COGNITIVE SCIENCES, 2005, 9 (06) :290-295